University at Albany, State University of New York University at Albany, State University of New York
Scholars Archive Scholars Archive
Legacy Theses & Dissertations (2009 - 2024) The Graduate School
1-1-2019
Adult children of divorce : how do attachment insecurity and Adult children of divorce : how do attachment insecurity and
interparental con<ict contribute to romantic relationship interparental con<ict contribute to romantic relationship
satisfaction? satisfaction?
Hannah Muetzelfeld
University at Albany, State University of New York
The University at Albany community has made this article openly available.
Please sharePlease share how this access bene;ts you.
Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.library.albany.edu/legacy-etd
Part of the Counseling Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Muetzelfeld, Hannah, "Adult children of divorce : how do attachment insecurity and interparental con<ict
contribute to romantic relationship satisfaction?" (2019).
Legacy Theses & Dissertations (2009 - 2024)
.
2531.
https://scholarsarchive.library.albany.edu/legacy-etd/2531
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the The Graduate School at Scholars Archive. It has
been accepted for inclusion in Legacy Theses & Dissertations (2009 - 2024) by an authorized administrator of
Scholars Archive.
Please see Terms of Use. For more information, please contact scholarsarchive@albany.edu.
!
i!
ADULT CHILDREN OF DIVORCE:
HOW DO ATTACHMENT INSECURITY AND INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT
CONTRIBUTE TO ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIP SATISFACTION?
by
Hannah K. Muetzelfeld
A Dissertation
Submitted to the University at Albany, State University of New York
in Partial Fulfillment of
the Requirements for the Degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
School of Education
Department of Educational & Counseling Psychology
2019
!
ii!
Acknowledgments
It certainly takes a village to make it to this point. First and foremost, I want to thank my
advisor and dissertation chair, Dr. Micki Friedlander. You graciously guided and supported me
throughout each step of this program, encouraged my confidence and autonomy in research, and
helped expand my knowledge of both couples research and therapy. I am a better writer,
researcher, and therapist because of you. Second, I want to thank Dr. Kimberly Colvin for
making statistics approachable, and for your patience and enthusiasm when answering my many
questions. Third, my thanks go to Dr. Laurie Heatherington for your willingness and eagerness to
be a part of my dissertation journey.
I would also like to express my gratitude for three individuals earlier on in my graduate
studies journey who were instrumental in my development. Dr. Rockey Robbins, thank you for
opening my eyes to the world of social injustice, and for always being willing to engage in
difficult and important dialogue. Dr. Paula McWhirter, thank you for providing me with
invaluable opportunities and support to begin my journey into academic writing and publishing.
And third, Dr. Cal Stoltenberg, thank you for helping me to develop my very first research idea,
which was certainly a springboard to the research I pursue today.
No time in a doctoral program is complete without friendships made along the way. I feel
so thankful to have had such a wonderful and supportive cohort (Alex Agiliga, Jennifer Bordon,
Melissa Ertl, Kieran Maestro, Christina Martin, and Englann Taylor). The potluck and game
nights we shared always rejuvenated my spirit. I also want to thank Larissa Barbaro-Kukade,
Nicole Da Silva, and Lynsay Paiko for being wonderful sources of support, laughter, and
friendship. I am so grateful for each of you.
Furthermore, I would like to express my thanks to family, whose support was there from
!
iii!
the beginning. Much gratitude and appreciation goes to my brother, Ken Allison, whose
kindness, generosity, and success have always served as an inspiration and guiding light to me.
Your driving force is one I can only hope to match. I am so thankful to call you brother and
friend. To my mother, Diane Adams, your strength, resilience, courage, and giving spirit have
always motivated and inspired me. I can never thank you enough for always being there to take
my call, and for believing that I could make it this far. You are, and always will be, my hero.
And lastly, to my partner, Kyle McCarthy, whose unrelenting and unconditional support has
lifted me more times than I can count. For your understanding nature, endless patience, kind
heart, and willingness to serve as my own personal editor – without you I could not have made it
through this program. I love you equal. Here’s to our next adventure!
!
!
iv!
Table of Contents
Acknowledgments .......................................................................................................................... ii
Table of Contents........................................................................................................................... iv
Abstract.......................................................................................................................................... vi
Chapter One: Introduction and Literature Review ........................................................................1
Attachment Theory ..................................................................................................3
Bowen Theory..........................................................................................................4
Hypotheses...............................................................................................................6
Mediation .....................................................................................................6
Moderation...................................................................................................8
Chapter Two: Method...................................................................................................................10
Participants.............................................................................................................10
Participant Characteristics .....................................................................................10
Instruments.............................................................................................................11
Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict.........................................11
Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised.............................................13
Couple Satisfaction Index-4.......................................................................14
Screening and Demographic Questionnaires.............................................15
Procedure ...............................................................................................................15
Chapter Three: Results...................................................................................................................17
Preliminary Analyses.............................................................................................17
Descriptive Statistics..............................................................................................17
Comparisons of the Study Variables with Previous Samples................................18
!
v!
Major Analyses ......................................................................................................18
Test of Mediation (Model 1)......................................................................18
Test of Moderation (Model 2) ...................................................................20
Chapter Four: Discussion...............................................................................................................22
Practical Implications ............................................................................................26
Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research.......................................26
References......................................................................................................................................29
List of Tables
Table 1. Participant Characteristics .......................................................................39
Table 2. Descriptive Statistics ...............................................................................41
Table 3. Conditional Effects of Interparental Conflict on Romantic Relationship
Satisfaction at Different Levels of Attachment Avoidance ...................................42
List of Figures
Figure 1. Hypothesized Mediation Model .............................................................43
Figure 2. Hypothesized Moderation Model...........................................................44
Appendices
Appendix A. Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale......................45
Appendix B. Experiences in Close Relationships – Revised.................................48
Appendix C. Couples Satisfaction Index – 4.........................................................50
Appendix D. Screening Questions.........................................................................51
Appendix E. Demographic Questionnaire .............................................................52
Appendix F. Informed Consent Document............................................................55
!
vi!
Abstract
Research has shown that children of divorce who are exposed to high levels of
interparental conflict tend to have worse adult outcomes than individuals not so exposed (e.g.,
Gager, Yabiku, & Linver, 2016), including damage to their romantic relationships (Cui,
Fincham, & Durtschi, 2011; Feeney, 2006). The present study investigated the contributing role
of adult attachment insecurity (i.e., attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety) to the relation
between recollections of interparental conflict during childhood and adult romantic relationship
satisfaction.
A convenience sample of 678 U.S. participants (319 men, 345 women) whose parents
had divorced prior to their reaching age 18 completed the Children’s Perception of Interparental
Conflict (CPIC; Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992), the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised
(Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000), and the Couples Satisfaction Index-4 (CSI-4; Funk & Rogge,
2007). Two theorized models were tested with Avoidance and Anxiety as moderators and
mediators using Hayes’s (2017) PROCESS macro.
Results showed that Avoidance, controlling for Anxiety, significantly moderated the
relation between CPIC and CSI-4 scores, where highly avoidant participants who recalled high
levels of interparental conflict in childhood reported significantly less adult relationship
satisfaction. Moreover, a post-hoc analysis showed that Avoidance partially mediated the
relation between Threat (a CPIC subscale) and relationship satisfaction. No gender differences
were found, and Anxiety was neither a significant moderator nor a mediator.
Contrary to previous studies (e.g., Cusimano & Riggs, 2013), participants’ retrospective
reports of interparental conflict were not significantly associated with attachment insecurity,
likely due to the narrower sampling of adults whose parents had divorced during their childhood.
!
This result calls into question the widely held belief that divorce, when paired with high levels of
interparental conflict, leads to poor relationship adjustment. Rather, the present findings suggest
that this outcome is most likely for adults with high levels of attachment avoidance, which may
develop when children feel threatened by interparental conflict.
Taken together, the present results support previous research indicating that avoidance is
the more problematic dimension of attachment insecurity in romantic relationships (Li & Chan,
2012) and the more difficult dimension to modify in psychotherapy (Wiseman & Tishby, 2014).
!
1!
Chapter 1
Introduction and Literature Review
Around half of all marriages in the U.S. end in divorce (Cherlin, 2010). The
multigenerational effects of divorce are reflected in numerous studies indicating that compared to
adults from two-parent families of origin, individuals whose parents divorced tend to have less
satisfying and long-lasting relationships (Cui & Fincham, 2010; Cui, Fincham, & Durtschi, 2011;
Jacquet & Surra, 2001). For this reason, it is important to identify factors that contribute to
troubled romantic relationships for these adults.
One contributing factor has been identified in numerous studies: the extent of conflict in
the parental relationship (Cui et al., 2011; Cusimano & Riggs, 2013; Feeney, 2006). Specifically,
research has shown that problematic romantic relationships in adulthood are linked to extensive
interparental conflict during childhood. Studies on interparental conflict, defined in terms of
frequency, intensity, and resolution of the conflict (Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992), indicate that
adult children from high-conflict families tend to report more attachment insecurity (Cusimano
& Riggs, 2013); less happiness (Amato, Loomis, & Booth, 1995); and shorter (Gager, Yabiku, &
Linver, 2016), poorer quality (Feeney, 2006), and less committed romantic relationships (Cui et
al., 2011).
Notably, the conflict that a person was exposed to between parents during childhood
seems to carry over to adult relationships, which tend to be of poor quality (Cui & Fincham,
2010; Cui, Fincham, & Pasley, 2008). Supporting this reasoning, a 12-year longitudinal study
found that children’s long-term outcomes (i.e., levels of psychological distress and overall
happiness) were associated with the extent of interparental conflict occurring prior to parental
divorce (Amato et al., 1995). Indeed, the extent of interparental conflict, which is a critical
!
2!
predictor of the impact of divorce on children (Amato et al., 1995; Gager et al., 2016), may be a
more important determinant of adult relationship outcomes than whether or not the parents
actually divorced (Hayashi & Strickland, 1998). Cui et al. (2008), for example, found that
interparental conflict, but not divorce, was associated with high rates of negative conflict
behaviors in young adults’ romantic relationships. In a subsequent study, participants who
recalled high pre-divorce conflict reported generally more favorable views about divorce as a
relationship outcome, as well as less stability in their own romantic relationships (Cui et al.,
2011).
The general aim of the present study was to contribute to the literature on the long-term
relationship outcomes of adults whose parents divorced during their childhood, not only in
relation to the extent of interparental conflict they recalled, but also in terms of their levels of
attachment anxiety and avoidance (i.e., attachment insecurity). Specifically, attachment
insecurity, a well-known contributor to romantic relationship quality (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Li
& Chan, 2012; Tucker & Anders, 1999), was investigated as a contributing factor to the long-
term effects of interparental conflict in childhood. Whereas previous studies have included
samples with adult children from both two-parent and divorced families (Cui & Fincham, 2010;
Cui et al., 2008), this study investigated only adult children of divorce in order to better
understand this phenomenon in that particular population, where levels of interparental conflict
might be more frequent or intense (Cui et al., 2011).
Numerous studies reported significant associations among attachment insecurity,
interparental conflict, and romantic relationship satisfaction (Cusimano & Riggs, 2013; Platt,
Nalbone, Casanova, & Wetchler, 2008; Steinberg, Davila, & Fincham, 2006). Generally, results
of these studies indicate that adults who were raised in homes with extensive interparental
!
3!
conflict tend to report high levels of problematic attachment, particularly jealousy and fears of
abandonment (Hayashi & Strickland, 1998). The way in which attachment insecurity contributes
to poor quality relationships for adults raised in high-conflict divorced families has yet to be
clarified, however.
As first described by Bowlby (1958), attachment security was viewed as largely resulting
from a strong emotional bond between mothers and children. Later on, Bowlby (1969) postulated
that children who experience a warm and loving relationship with their mothers develop positive
internal working models of self and others that endure throughout life. Having a favorable view
of self allows children to take risks and view themselves as worthy of love (Bartholomew, 1990).
In contrast, attachment insecurity, which develops from poor parental relationships, is theorized
to result in a negative working model, i.e., seeing the self as unlovable and unworthy of love and
support from others (Bartholomew, 1990; Bretherton & Munholland, 2008). In sum, the kind of
internal working model developed in childhood is theorized to have a longstanding effect on how
adults feel and behave long after they have left their families of origin (Bowlby, 1979).
Supporting this theory, Hazan and Shaver (1987) found that attachment behaviors tend to
endure over time. Furthermore, a person’s style of attachment, or characteristic way of behaving
with close others (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978), tends to remain constant into
adulthood, particularly affecting the person’s romantic relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). In
other words, individuals who did not experience a safe, nurturing environment in childhood are
likely to develop a negative internal working model of self and others, which is reflected in high
attachment insecurity in their adult romantic relationships (Owen & Cox, 1997).
It was reasoned that although a child’s lack of safety with others can be due to a number
of factors, safety is likely to be diminished in high-conflict homes. Negligent parenting may also
!
4!
result in children developing high attachment anxiety, leading them to constantly seek attention,
reassurance, and approval from others. Conversely, these children might develop high
attachment avoidance, leading them to distance emotionally from others in order to protect
themselves from rejection.
The attachment construct has been conceptualized both categorically, as one of several
characteristic styles, or dimensionally along two continua of insecurity, attachment avoidance
and attachment anxiety (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Theoretically,
people with high attachment avoidance tend to see others as unreliable and untrustworthy,
whereas people with high attachment anxiety tend to see themselves as unlovable (Bartholomew,
1990; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). As a consequence of these views of self and others,
people high in attachment avoidance tend to be emotionally unavailable to others in order to
avoid conflict and/or rejection (Feeney & Karantzas, 2017; Hazan & Shaver, 1994), whereas
people high in attachment anxiety tend to seek intense emotional closeness and are overly fearful
of abandonment (Hazan & Shaver, 1994).
The advantage of conceptualizing attachment dimensionally is the ability to measure
levels of attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety for each individual. It is possible to
develop high levels of both attachment anxiety and avoidance (Brennan et al., 1998); indeed,
these two attachment dimensions tend to be moderately associated (Butzer & Campbell, 2008;
Fraley, 2012; Woodhouse, Dykas, & Cassidy, 2009). Alternately, a person could develop high
attachment avoidance with little anxiety or vice versa. For this reason, the present study
investigated the two dimensions as joint contributors to relational outcomes, as well as each
dimension considered independently of the other.
In addition to attachment theory, the present study was further informed by Bowen’s
!
5!
(1978; Kerr & Bowen, 1988) family systems theory, which explains the quality of family
functioning over multiple generations, particularly in response to stress. According to this theory,
severe problems, particularly in a family that experiences high parental conflict and/or divorce,
invariably affect children into adulthood through what Bowen (1978) termed the nuclear family
projection process. That is, in response to repeated strife between parents, children may become
“fused” with one parent or cut themselves off emotionally from one or both parents.
Fusion and emotional cutoff are opposing aspects of poor differentiation of self, the major
construct in family systems theory that explains adults’ levels of functioning as due to the family
projection process experienced in childhood (Bowen, 1978). Poor self-differentiation has
repeatedly been associated with adults’ psychological distress and dissatisfying romantic
relationships (Lampis, 2016; Skowron, 2000; Skowron & Friedlander, 1998). Pertinent to the
present study, Priest (2015) found that differentiation of self accounted for the association
between experiences of family violence and romantic relationship distress. That is, high family
violence predicted lower levels of differentiation, which in turn predicted greater relationship
distress. In several studies of couple adjustment, high differentiation of self was associated with
greater romantic relationship satisfaction (Ferreira, Narciso, Novo, & Pereira, 2014; Skowron &
Friedlander, 1998), whereas the emotional cutoff aspect of differentiation was associated with
poor relationship adjustment (Banse, 2004; Peleg, 2008; Simpson 1990; Skowron, 2000).
The conceptual similarity between attachment security and high self-differentiation was
supported in an investigation of married adults (Timm & Keiley, 2011). More specifically,
fusion, or emotional reactivity, is similar to attachment anxiety (Skowron & Dendy, 2004) in that
both constructs refer to an overreliance on the thoughts and feelings of others, particularly
romantic partners or other close family members. On the other hand, emotional cutoff, or
!
6!
reactive distancing, is similar to attachment avoidance (Skowron & Dendy, 2004) in that both of
these constructs refer to removing oneself (emotionally or literally) from others, particularly
when a close relationship is tense or conflictual (Kerr & Bowen, 1988).
This reasoning has been supported in several studies that examined family systems and
attachment constructs concurrently. Skowron and Dendy (2004), for example, found that
attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety were significantly associated with emotional cutoff
and emotional reactivity, respectively. Similarly, Lopez (2001) found that attachment anxiety
was associated with greater emotional reactivity (i.e., fusion), and associated with less self-other
differentiation. Another study (Wei, Vogel, Ku, & Zakalik, 2005) found that the relation between
attachment anxiety and psychological and interpersonal distress was mediated by emotional
reactivity, whereas the relation between attachment avoidance and distress was mediated by
emotional cutoff.
The Present Study
The present study investigated two ways in which attachment insecurity may contribute
to the relation between adult romantic relationship satisfaction and childhood experiences of
interparental conflict. Specifically, two hypotheses were tested. First, attachment anxiety and
avoidance were hypothesized to mediate the relation between the extent of interparental conflict
recalled during childhood and participants’ reported romantic relationship satisfaction. Second,
attachment anxiety and avoidance were hypothesized to moderate this relation. The two
theorized models are depicted in Figures 1 and 2.
Mediation. First, it was reasoned that as a mediator, attachment insecurity (anxiety and
avoidance) would at least partially account for the negative association between interparental
conflict and relationship satisfaction. Based on attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969) and family
!
7!
systems theory (Bowen, 1978), the experience of intense interparental conflict in childhood may
lead a child to develop a high level of attachment insecurity, which in adulthood is may
compromise the person’s ability to sustain a high quality, satisfying romantic relationship.
The available literature supports the hypothesized relations between attachment
insecurity and (a) interparental conflict (Cusimano & Riggs, 2013; Platt et al., 2008) as well as
(b) romantic relationship satisfaction (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Stackert & Bursik, 2003). In the
present study, both attachment avoidance and anxiety were posited as mediators, since the
literature provided no guidance for predicting which dimension of attachment insecurity in adult
relationships might be an outcome of interparental conflict. Whereas some studies reported that
interparental conflict was only associated with attachment anxiety (Hayashi & Strickland, 1998;
Rodrigues & Kitzmann, 2007), other studies indicated significant associations with both
dimensions of insecurity (e.g., Cusimano & Riggs, 2013). In a sample of college students, for
example, the frequency, intensity, and resolution of interparental conflict were associated with
both dimensions (Platt et al., 2008). Additionally, in a study of adolescent girls, both attachment
avoidance (e.g., avoidance of intimacy) and anxiety (e.g. fears of abandonment) mediated the
association between interparental conflict and participants’ expectations of unhappiness in future
relationships (Steinberg et al., 2006). !
The association between romantic relationship satisfaction and attachment insecurity has
also been repeatedly supported. Overall, insecurely attached individuals are less likely to be
satisfied in their romantic relationships than individuals who are securely attached (Banse, 2004;
Feeney & Noller, 1990). It seems that the two dimensions of attachment insecurity may operate
differently, however. In a meta-analytic review (Li & Chan, 2012), although both dimensions of
insecurity had strong relations with relationship quality and satisfaction, the association is
!
8!
generally stronger for avoidance than for anxiety. On the other hand, compared to avoidance,
attachment anxiety tends to be associated with more conflictual romantic relationships. There
also may be gender differences, since one study reported that attachment anxiety was associated
with less relationship satisfaction for both men and women, whereas attachment avoidance was
negatively associated with satisfaction only for the men in the sample (Tucker & Anders, 1999).
Moderation. Second, it was hypothesized that as a moderator, attachment insecurity
would strengthen the negative association between interparental conflict in childhood and
romantic relationship satisfaction in adulthood. In contrast to the mediation model, in this model
attachment insecurity is not viewed as temporally linked to interparental conflict. It was reasoned
that insecurity might not necessarily be due to having been raised in a high-conflict divorced
family, but rather may be due to other experiences in childhood, such as parental neglect, or
adulthood, such as trauma. Moreover, adults may develop secure attachments to others even in
the face of extreme interparental conflict if their bonds with one or both parents are strong and
secure or if, as adults, they are able to develop high quality relationships with others in their
lives.
It was reasoned that combined with high interparental conflict in divorced families, high
attachment insecurity may be a long-term risk factor for adults’ romantic relationships, whereas
low levels of attachment insecurity (i.e., greater attachment security) may buffer the influence of
childhood interparental conflict on romantic relationship satisfaction in adulthood. Supporting
this reasoning, one study (Hare, Miga, & Allen, 2009) found that high attachment insecurity
strengthened the association between interparental aggression and participants’ dating
aggression. Additionally, El-Sheikh and Elmore-Staton (2004) reported that that a secure bond
between father and child buffered the negative effects of interparental conflict on children’s
!
9!
externalizing behaviors. Since adult children of divorce tend to have longer lasting relationships
if they are securely attached (Crowell, Treboux, & Brockmeyer, 2009), it may be that low
attachment insecurity disrupts the multigenerational transmission of poor relationship
adjustment.
As in the mediation model, it seemed possible that anxiety and avoidance could operate
differently in interaction with interparental conflict. Grych and Kinsfogel (2010), for example,
found that in adolescents, both dimensions of attachment insecurity moderated the relation
between experiences of family aggression and negative dating behaviors, although a gender
difference emerged. Whereas high attachment anxiety in boys strengthened the relation between
interparental conflict and dating aggression, high avoidance and anxiety in girls strengthened the
relation between interparental conflict and abusive dating behaviors.
!
10!
Chapter 2
Method
Participants
Volunteers residing in the U.S. were recruited to take part in a web-based study on
“individuals who experienced parental divorce in childhood.Inclusion criteria were (a) being 18
years of age or older, (b) having parents who had divorced when the participant was between the
ages of 6 and 17, (c) currently being in a romantic relationship for at least 3 months. The last
criterion was used to ensure an established romantic relationship (e.g., Campbell, Simpson,
Boldry, & Kashy, 2005). It was reasoned that children whose parents divorced after age 5 were
likely aware of their parents’ conflict, and the age limit of 17 was reasoned to restrict the sample
to participants who had experienced divorce in childhood. An a priori power analysis was
conducted based on effect sizes calculated from previous relevant literature, a familywise error
of α = .025, and statistical power of .95. To detect a medium effect size to test 2 hypotheses, at
least 280 participants were needed.
Among the 806 participants who completed the research materials, 27 omitted more than
5% of the survey data,. An additional 99 participants completed the study despite not meeting the
inclusion criteria, and 2 participants responded incorrectly to the validity check questions in the
survey. Consequently, all of these 128 participants were excluded from the analyses, following
recommendations made by Yeatts & Martin (2015), resulting in a final sample of N = 678.
Participant characteristics. As shown in Table 1, the average age of participants in the
final data set was 34 years (M = 33.6, SD = 10.5; range 18-71). Among the 678 participants, 319
(47.1%) identified as men and 345 (50.9%) as women. Participants identified as White (n = 450,
66.4%), Asian/Asian American (n = 73, 10.8%), Black/African American (n = 51, 7.5%),
Hispanic/Latin American (n = 36, 5.3%), Native American/American Indian (n = 19, 2.8%), or
!
11!
biracial/multiracial (n = 31, 4.6%). While the birth country of most participants was the U.S. (n =
595, 87.8%), the remaining participants listed birth countries in every continent. In terms of
education, most participants had a bachelor’s degree or higher (n = 342, 50.4%) and an annual
household income under $75,000 (n = 468, 69%).
Most participants were not married (n = 363, 53.5%). The length of participants’ current
relationships was M = 5.4 years, Mdn = 3, SD = 6.9; range 0.3 to 45.4. On average, participants
had one child (SD = 1.3; range 0-8). Most participants identified as heterosexual/straight (n =
546, 80.5%), while 10.5% (n = 71) identified as bisexual, 3.4% (n = 23) as gay or lesbian, and
2.4% (n = 16) as pansexual.
The average age at which participants’ parents had divorced was 11 years (M = 10.9, SD
= 3.4; range 6-17). The majority of participants reported that their mother had legal custody of
them following divorce (n = 398, 58.7%); 23.7% (n = 161) reported that their parents had joint
custody, and 11.2% (n = 76) reported that their father was their legal guardian. Furthermore,
most participants lived primarily with their mother after the divorce (n = 474, 69.9%); 15 % (n =
102) lived with their fathers.
Instruments
Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict. The Conflict Properties (CP) scale in
the Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict scale (CPIC; Grych et al., 1992; see Appendix
A) measured the frequency, intensity, and resolution of interparental conflict recalled in
childhood. The 48-item CPIC assesses 8 conflict dimensions: frequency, intensity, resolution,
content, perceived threat, coping efficacy, self-blame, and triangulation. These dimensions
comprise three broader domains of interparental conflict: Conflict Properties (19 items;
!
12!
Frequency, Intensity, and Resolution), Threat (12 items; Perceived Threat and Coping Efficacy),
and Self-Blame (9 items; Content and Self-Blame).
Since the present study examined retrospective reports of interparental conflict, the verb
tense was changed from present tense to past tense. Retrospective reports on the CPIC have
previously been used, with adequate reliability (α = .88, Bickham & Fiese, 1997). CP items
include, “I never saw my parents arguing or disagreeing” (Frequency), “My parents have broken
or thrown things during an argument” (Intensity), and “When my parents had an argument they
usually worked it out” (Resolution) (Bickham & Fiese, 1997; Grych et al., 1992, p. 570).
Items are rated on a 3-point Likert-type scale (0 = true, 1 = sort of or sometimes true, 2 =
false) in relation to the experience of interparental conflict in childhood. After 8 items are reverse
scored, the raw CP scores are averaged to range from 0 to 2. High CP scores indicate more
extensive interparental conflict. Although the entire scale was administered, initially only the
total Conflict Properties scale was used in the major analyses, since its three subscales
(Frequency, Intensity, and Resolution) were shown to measure a single underlying construct
(Grych et al., 1992). Subsequently, the Threat scale was used in a post hoc mediation analysis.
In terms of validity, the CPIC was significantly correlated with parent-rated measures of
marital conflict and aggression (Grych et al., 1992), including the O’Leary-Porter Scale (Porter
& O’Leary, 1980), r = .30, and the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1979), r = .39.The CPIC
demonstrated factor invariance among adolescents and emerging adults (Moura, dos Santos,
Rocha, & Matos, 2010), and evidenced high two-week test-retest reliability (r = .95; Bickham &
Fiese, 1997). The authors of the measure (Grych et al., 1992) reported internal consistency
reliabilities, αs = .90 and .89, for the Conflict Properties scale, comparable to values obtained by
Moura et al. (2010), α = .92. In the present sample, the internal consistency estimate for CP was
!
13!
α = .89.
Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised. The Anxiety and Avoidance scales in the
36-item Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R; Fraley et al., 2000; see Appendix
B) assessed participants’ self-reported levels of each dimension of attachment insecurity. The
ECR-R is a revision of the Experiences in Close Relationships scale (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998)
using Item Response Theory (Fraley et al., 2000).
Respondents are directed to respond to the ECR-R items with respect to their general
experiences with romantic partners. The Anxiety scale measures respondents’ level of security
about the availability of romantic partners, with higher scores indicating more anxiety about
partners’ availability and responsiveness, as well as a greater fear of abandonment. Sample items
include, “I worry that romantic partners won’t care about me as much as I care about them” and
“I worry a lot about my relationships.” The Avoidance scale measures comfort depending on or
being emotionally intimate with partners, with high scores indicating greater discomfort. Sample
items include, “I don't feel comfortable opening up to romantic partners” and “I get
uncomfortable when romantic partners want to be very close.” The 18 items on each scale are
rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). After several
items are reverse keyed, the raw scores are averaged, resulting in scores ranging from 1 – 7 for
each attachment dimension.
In terms of construct validity, the ECR-R scales were significantly correlated with the
Relationship Questionnaire (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), a different measure of
attachment, in a college sample: Anxiety r = .60, Avoidance r = .62 (Sibley et al., 2005). In
another study, Anxiety was associated with feelings of loneliness (r = .53) and worry (r = .39;
Fairchild & Finney, 2006), whereas Avoidance was significantly correlated with avoidance of
!
14!
touching in the context of romantic relationships (r = .51), and using touch to show affection (r =
-.51; Fairchild & Finney, 2006).
Fraley et al. (2000) reported test-retest reliability estimates for the Anxiety and
Avoidance scales as rs = .94 and .95, respectively. Internal consistency reliabilities were reported
as α = .91 (Anxiety) and .95 (Avoidance) by Cusimano and Riggs (2013); .92 (Anxiety) and .93
(Avoidance) by Fairchild and Finney (2006). Previous research has shown that the Anxious and
Avoidance scales are moderately correlated, with rs ranging from .41 to .58 (Fraley, Heffernan,
Vicary, & Brumbaugh, 2011; Sibley, Fischer, & Liu, 2005; Sibley & Liu, 2004). In the present
sample, the internal consistency estimate for the Anxiety scale was α = .94, while the internal
consistency for the Avoidance scale was α = .92.
Couples Satisfaction Index-4. The Couples Satisfaction Index-4 (CSI-4; Funk & Rogge,
2007; see Appendix C) was used to measure participants’ satisfaction with their current romantic
relationships. The CSI-4 is a shortened version of the 32-item Couples Satisfaction Index (Funk
& Rogge, 2007). Funk and Rogge used Item Response Theory to create the CSI-4, a more
parsimonious scale that assesses levels of warmth, happiness, and general satisfaction within a
romantic relationship.
One CSI-4 item is rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale, from 0 (extremely unhappy) to 6
(perfect); a second item is rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale, from 0 (not at all true) to 5
(completely true); and the two remaining items are rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale, from 0
(not at all) to 5 (completely). Scores on the 4 items are summed to create a total score that ranges
from 0 – 21, with higher scores indicating greater relationship satisfaction.
In terms of convergent validity, in an online community sample the CSI-4 was highly
correlated with three other relationship satisfaction measures (Funk and Rogge, 2007): the
!
15!
Marital Adjustment Test (r = .87; Locke & Wallace, 1959), the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (r =
.88; Spanier, 1976), and the Relationship Assessment Scale (r = .94; Hendrick, 1988).
Funk and Rogge reported the CSI-4’s internal consistency reliability as α = .94. A more recent
estimate was α = .93 (Cui & Fincham, 2010). In the present sample, the internal consistency
estimate was α = .93.
Screening and demographic questionnaires. Volunteers were screened for inclusion
using four questions related to the inclusion criteria (see Appendix D). At the end of the survey,
participants were asked to respond to a brief demographic questionnaire (see Appendix E), with
questions about their age, gender, partner’s gender, race/ethnicity, country of origin, household
income, education level, sexual orientation, relationship status, length of current romantic
relationship, age at which parents divorced, and primary custody and living arrangements
following the parents’ divorce.
Additionally, four items used by Bickham and Fiese (1997) were included to assess the
clarity of participants’ childhood memories of parental conflict, e.g., “I have clear memories
about my parents’ arguments.Each item was rated on a scale from 1 to 3, with a possible range
of 4 to 12. Lower scores indicate clearer memories. Total memory scores were correlated with
scores on Conflict Properties scale in a preliminary analysis.
Procedure
After approval by the university’s Institutional Review Board, participants were recruited
through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, an online crowdsourcing service that other researchers
found to provide valid community data (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Peer, Samat,
Brandimarte, & Acquisti, 2015). In order to ensure that roughly equal numbers of men and
women were collected for analyses, two gender-specific samples of roughly 350 participants
!
16!
were collected and then combined for analysis. Since it seemed that a sample of gender-
nonconforming participants would likely not be large enough for separate analyses, only men
and women were recruited for participation.
Volunteers were recruited to participate in a study on “childhood parental conflict and
adult romantic relationships.” Potential participants were directed to psychdata.com to review the
informed consent (see Appendix F), in which volunteers were informed that participation in the
study was voluntary and confidential, and that they had the right to withdraw at any point.
Volunteers consented to participate by clicking “continue” on the informed consent page,
after which they were directed to four screening questions (i.e., age, parental divorce status,
relationship status, and U.S. residency status). Participants who did not meet these four inclusion
criteria were directed to the end of the study and thanked for their time. Participants received
$0.25 for completion of the survey.
The ECR-R and CSI-4 were counterbalanced, followed by the CPIC and demographic
questionnaire. The CPIC was administered in the third position to avoid priming.
Several open-ended attention-check questions were added throughout the research
materials to identify possible cases of random responding. These items included, “In which
direction does the sun rise?” and “What color is the sky?”
!
17!
Chapter 3
Results
Preliminary Analyses
DFBETA values, a measure of the degree to which an observation influences the
regression line, were examined along with Cook’s distance, leverage, and external studentized
deleted residuals (SDRES) to determine outliers. No outliers or influential cases were identified
based on these criteria.
To assess for order effects, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was
conducted. No significant differences were found, Wilks’s λ = .99, F(4, 673) = .33.
All of the assumptions for multiple regression (multicollinearity, linearity, normality,
homoscedasticity, and independence of errors) were met. All skewness and kurtosis values were
within normal limits.
Descriptive Statistics
The means and standard deviations on the study variables, as well as their
intercorrelations, are reported in Table 2. The bivariate correlations indicated a direct relation
between Anxiety and Avoidance, r(676) = .54, p < .001, and inverse relations between each
attachment dimension and relationship satisfaction (CSI-4) rs(676) = -.34 and -.56, respectively.
As shown in the table, gender was not significantly associated with either attachment
dimension or with CSI-4, rs(664) = -.04 to .07. For this reason, gender was not included in the
tests of the moderation and mediation models.
Total Memory scores, which indicated fairly clear memories (M = 7.00, SD = 1.92, range
4-12), were significantly correlated with scores on the Conflict Properties scale, r(676) = -.30, p
< .001 and with age at the time of parental divorce, r(676) = -.20, p < .001, although its
!
18!
reliability was modest, α = .49. This result indicated that participants who were younger at the
time of the divorce and recalled more conflict between their parents had clearer memories of the
events. Consequently, Memory was included as a potential covariate in the major analyses.
Comparisons of the Study Variables with Previous Samples
Comparisons of the mean CP score in the present sample with the means from other
samples that completed the measure retrospectively indicated significant differences.
Specifically, the present sample’s CP scores were significantly lower (M = 1.27, SD = 0.44) than
the scores reported by Moura et al. (2010), M = 2.83, SD = 0.94, and by Reese-Weber and
Hesson-McInnis (2008), M = 1.79, SD = 0.59, both ps < .001. Additionally, CSI-4 scores in the
present sample (M = 17.81, SD = 4.60) were significantly lower, p < .001, than those reported by
Cui et al. (2008) (M = 20.44, SD = 4.30), who examined the association between interparental
conflict and romantic relationship satisfaction.
Major Analyses
The two hypothesized models were tested independently. To ensure maximum statistical
power, the familywise error rate was .05, with each model tested at α
pc
= .025.
Memory was not found to be a significant covariate in either the moderation, b = .09, SE
= .08, t = 1.08, 95% BCa CI [-.07, .25] or the mediation, b = .08, SE = .10, t = .80, 95% BCa CI
[-.11, .26] model. For this reason, Memory was eliminated from the subsequent analyses.
Test of mediation (Model 1). Due to the inclusion of two mediators, a parallel multiple
mediator model (Hayes, 2017) was used. The analysis was conducted with CP scores as the
predictor variable, Avoidance and Anxiety scores as two parallel mediators, and CSI-4 scores as
the criterion variable. The model was tested in SPSS using Hayes’s (2017) PROCESS macro.
!
19!
Bootstrapped 95% bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) confidence intervals with 10,000
resamples were calculated to assess the significance of the indirect effects (Hayes, 2017). In this
approach, the difference between the two indirect effects is calculated, and a bootstrap
confidence interval for the difference is created.
Neither the overall model, F(2, 675) = 0.22, nor the indirect effects of Avoidance
Z = -.65, 95% BCa CI [-.71, .34] and Anxiety Z = -.47, 95% BCa CI [-.12, .02], was significant.
These results indicated that neither dimension of attachment insecurity mediated the relation
between interparental conflict and romantic relationship satisfaction.
It was reasoned that because CP scores did not significantly predict Avoidance or
Anxiety, it seemed possible that the development of attachment avoidance could be due to one
aspect of interparental conflict, perceived threat. That is, how threatened or unsafe a child feels
by the interparental conflict may account for the development of attachment insecurity, resulting
in poor relationship outcomes in adulthood.
For this reason, the mediating effect of attachment insecurity was tested in a post-hoc
analysis between Threat (a CPIC subscale) and relationship satisfaction. In order to minimize
Type I error, bootstrapped 99% BCa confidence intervals were calculated with 10,000 resamples
(Hayes, 2017). In the CPIC, Threat assesses the extent to which participants felt threatened by
their parents’ conflict and unable to cope with the experience (Grych et al., 1992). Results of this
PROCESS analysis indicated a significant standardized indirect effect for Avoidance, b = -.06,
BCa CI [-0.13, -0.004], but not for Anxiety. In other words, only Avoidance partially accounted
for the relation between perceptions of threat of the interparental conflict in childhood and
romantic relationship satisfaction in adulthood.
!
20!
Test of moderation (Model 2). Due to the inclusion of two moderators, an additive
multiple moderation model was tested (Hayes, 2017), with CP scores as the predictor variable,
Avoidance and Anxiety scores as the moderators, and CSI-4 scores as the criterion variable. As
with the mediation model, the moderation model was analyzed in SPSS using PROCESS (Hayes,
2017).
Although the overall moderation model was significant, F(8, 669) = 47.77, p < .001, the
interaction terms were not (CP X Avoidance, b = -.34, SE = .43, t = -1.02, 95% BCa CI [-1.2,
.37]; CP X Anxiety, b = -.02, SE = .31, t = -.06, 95% BCa CI [-.62, .59]). Due to the moderate,
significant correlation. r(676) = .54, p < .001, between Anxiety and Avoidance, the moderating
effects of each attachment dimension were re-analyzed, with each moderator controlling for the
other.
First, the model with Avoidance as the moderator (controlling for Anxiety) indicated a
significant overall effect, F(4, 673) = 90.04, p < .001. The interaction term accounted for a
significant portion of variance in romantic relationship satisfaction, CP X Avoidance b = -.60, SE
= .27, t = -2.2, p > .05, 95% BCa CI [-1.1, -.07]. Second, the moderation test with Anxiety
(controlling for Avoidance) was not significant, CP X Anxiety b = -.17, SE = .15, t = -1.2, 95%
BCa CI [-.45, .11].
Due to the nonsignificant correlation between CP and CSI-4, r (676) = -.04, the
significant moderation effect for Avoidance was closely examined. Specifically, the Johnson-
Neyman technique was used to identify the point(s) along the continuous moderators at which
the relation between CP and CSI-4 transitioned from nonsignificant to significant at α = .05.
Results showed that the moderation of CP by Avoidance was in the hypothesized direction at
higher levels of Avoidance (see Table 3). Specifically, the conditional effect transitioned to
!
21!
statistical significance at an Avoidance score of 5.06, b = -1.36, SE = .69, t = -1.96, p < .05, 95%
BCa CI [-2.72, .00], which was at the 94
th
percentile of the distribution. In other words,
controlling for Anxiety, high Avoidance (above 5.06 on a 7-point scale) significantly
strengthened the inverse relation between interparental conflict and romantic relationship
satisfaction.
!
22!
Chapter 4
Discussion
Previous research has not clearly explicated how attachment difficulties play a role in the
romantic relationships of adults whose parents divorced during childhood. The present study
contributes to this literature by elucidating for whom and how, (i.e., with self-reported
attachment avoidance and anxiety as moderators and mediators) the childhood experience of
interparental conflict prior to divorce contributes to relationship dissatisfaction in adulthood.
Results suggest that attachment avoidance, rather than attachment anxiety, is the greater
risk factor. First, as a moderator, high levels of avoidance (controlling for anxiety), combined
with memories of high interparental conflict during childhood, significantly predicted
relationship dissatisfaction. In contrast, the moderation analysis with high attachment anxiety
(controlling for avoidance) was not significant. Second, while neither attachment dimension
significantly mediated the relation between interparental conflict and relationship satisfaction, a
post-hoc analysis showed that when feeling threatened and unable to cope (i.e., a consequence of
interparental conflict) was the predictor, attachment avoidance (but not attachment anxiety) had a
significant indirect effect on relationship satisfaction. This finding is in line with previous
research, where attachment avoidance was found to mediate the relation between experiences of
childhood abuse and poor adult relationships (McCarthy & Taylor, 1999).
Taken together, these findings suggest that high attachment avoidance is a notable
contributor to poor quality relationships among adults whose parents divorced during childhood,
whether the avoidance developed as a result of feeling threatened during the parents’ arguments
or originated in some other way. Moreover, results of the mediation analyses suggest that a
distinction should be made between recalling interparental conflict and feeling threatened by the
!
23!
experience. In other words, it is not merely the presence of interparental conflict, but rather its
impact on the child that seems to account for the development of attachment avoidance in
adulthood and its consequent influence on relationship adjustment.
One explanation for the difference between these results and some previous research in
which interparental conflict was associated with relationship dissatisfaction (Cui & Fincham,
2010; Cui et al., 2008) may be due in part to the narrower criteria for inclusion in the present
sample. In the studies by Cui and colleagues, participants included adult children from two-
parent as well as divorced families, whereas the present study only sampled adult children whose
parents divorced before they reached age 18. Notably, in a later study Cui et al. (2011) found that
interparental conflict experienced by adult children of divorce was not associated with
relationship commitment, a construct closely related to satisfaction (Cui & Fincham, 2010;
Hendrick, Hendrick, & Adler, 1988).
One distinctive aspect of this study was the analysis of each attachment dimension
controlling for the other. The finding that attachment avoidance was a uniquely significant
moderator is consistent with a meta-analysis (Li & Chan, 2012), in which romantic relationship
satisfaction was more strongly linked with attachment avoidance than with attachment anxiety.
Consideration of the unique contribution of each attachment insecurity dimension may explain
some mixed findings in the literature. Whereas one previous study found that both attachment
avoidance and anxiety strengthened associations between experiences of interparental conflict
and adolescent girls’ abusive dating behaviors (Grych & Kinsfogel, 2010), another study found
that attachment avoidance (but not anxiety) strengthened the association of experiences of child
abuse and adults’ symptoms of post-traumatic stress (Busuito, Huth-Bocks, & Puro, 2014).
!
24!
The bivariate associations between each dimension of attachment and relationship
satisfaction were significant in the predicted direction, Anxiety = -.34, Avoidance = -.56. These
results are consistent with numerous previous studies in which both avoidance (Li & Chan, 2012;
Tucker & Anders, 1999) and anxiety (Birnbaum, 2007; Li & Chan, 2012) were associated with
poor romantic relationship satisfaction. These results also support theorizing (Hazan & Shaver,
1994) that individuals’ characteristic attachment insecurity, especially attachment avoidance, is
likely to compromise the ability to develop and sustain satisfying romantic relationships in
adulthood.
In contrast to previous research (Cusimano & Riggs, 2013; Platt et al., 2008), however,
the frequency, intensity, and resolution of interparental conflict occurring during participants’
childhood was not significantly associated with either attachment avoidance or attachment
anxiety. This result is in opposition to the widespread notion that exposure to interparental
conflict in childhood contributes to poor relationship adjustment in adulthood. Instead, the
present results indicate that poor relationship satisfaction may be more closely tied to the
development of attachment avoidance, which may result from feeling threatened by interparental
conflict.
In addition to attachment theory, the present study was informed by Bowen’s (1978)
family systems theory, in which the concept of multigenerational transmission refers to family
dysfunction being repeated over generations. This projection process is said to occur when adults
who had witnessed significant interparental conflict as children become emotionally cut off or
fused with their romantic partners; both responses are considered to reflect dysfunction. The
post-hoc results partially support this theoretical assertion in that recall of a highly conflictual
!
25!
relationship between parents who divorced only contributed to participants’ poor relationship
adjustment if they had felt threatened and unable to cope.
The present study was designed to improve on research in this area in two ways. First, in
contrast to a categorical measurement of attachment (e.g., Feeney & Noller, 1990; Steinberg et
al., 2006), which restricts the sample to participants who report being either avoidant or anxious,
a dimensional approach allows for the inclusion of participants who reported being both highly
anxious and highly avoidant or neither (i.e., highly secure) in the analyses. Reflecting the
importance of this methodological distinction, the present results indicated a significant
difference in the unique moderating contribution of each attachment dimension.
Second, most studies on relations between interparental conflict, attachment insecurity,
and romantic relationship satisfaction were conducted with non-diverse college samples or fairly
homogeneous adult samples (Camisasca, Miragoli, Di Blasio, & Grych, 2017; Cui & Fincham,
2010; Cusimano & Riggs, 2013; Hayashi & Strickland, 1998; Platt et al., 2008; Rodrigues &
Kitzmann, 2007; Steinberg et al., 2006). In contrast, the present sample was more heterogeneous
in terms of race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, and age, thereby maximizing the generalizability of
the study’s findings. On the other hand, the sample only included adult children of divorce,
which restricted the external validity of the results while increasing its internal validity.
With respect to age, many studies of interparental conflict only surveyed adolescents and
young adults (Bickham & Fiese, 1997; Moura et al., 2010; Reese-Weber & Hesson-McInnis,
2008). Participants in these studies had conflict scores that were significantly higher than those
of the present participants, who had an average age of 33.6 (range 18 to 71) and were thus more
distant from their childhood experiences. The present participants also reported significantly less
!
26!
relationship satisfaction than the notably younger participants in Cui et al. (2018), possibly due
to the length of the romantic relationships reported by the present sample (M = 5.4 years).
Practical Implications
In terms of implications for clinical practice, understanding the importance of attachment
avoidance in adults from high-conflict divorced families suggests an additional avenue for
exploration when working individually with adults and youth whose romantic relationships are in
distress. Focusing on developing greater attachment security (e.g., reducing attachment
avoidance) may lessen the negative long-lasting impact of interparental conflict, improve a
client’s ability to develop satisfying relationships in adulthood, and increase general well being.
Additionally, the present results suggest that therapists who work with couples should
directly target attachment avoidance, which is manifested in emotional unavailability and an
avoidance of conflict (Feeney & Karantzas, 2017; Hazan & Shaver, 1994), or what Gottman
(1994) called stonewalling (cf. Fowler & Dillow, 2011). Since emotionally focused couple
therapy (Johnson, 2008) uses attachment to conceptualize couples’ interactions, this model of
treatment seems particularly suitable for treating clients who demonstrate high levels of
attachment avoidance.
Furthermore, the results of this study have implications for prevention. That is, group
psychoeducation for adolescents whose parents are separated or divorced could focus on healthy
relationship communication, emotion regulation, and attachment security. By working to buffer
the effects of interparental conflict on attachment avoidance, children of high-conflict divorce
may experience greater relationship satisfaction in adulthood.
Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research
!
27!
The use of Mechanical Turk to collect data required all participants needed to have
computer access, as well as a reasonable amount of computer literacy, affecting the external
validity of the present results. Furthermore, all measures were self-report, introducing mono-
method bias and common method variance. Due to the voluntary nature of the study and the
resulting convenience sample, self-selection bias may have threatened internal validity. Perhaps
individuals who recalled a great deal of conflict between their parents chose not to participate
due to the trauma experienced as a result of the conflict.
Additionally, the directions on the CPIC asked participants to report their retrospective
perceptions of interparental conflict. It is possible that these reports were not accurate. Future
studies should consider including the reports from both self and parents in order to obtain a more
accurate picture of interparental conflict. Alternately, self-report data can be collected
longitudinally, from adolescence to adulthood.
With respect to gender, an attempt was made to recruit roughly equal numbers of men
and women participants. The nonsignificant gender difference in satisfaction is consistent with
results from several studies (Cui et al., 2008, 2010; Gleeson & Fitzgerald, 2014), whereas the
nonsignificant gender differences in attachment avoidance and anxiety are consistent with some
(Shi, 2003; Stackert & Bursik, 2003) but not other studies (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991;
Brennan et al., 1998; Kirkpatrick & Davis, 1994; Scharfe & Bartholomew, 1994), in which
women reported greater attachment anxiety and men reported greater attachment avoidance.
Future research on attachment and relationship satisfaction should be extended to non-binary and
genderqueer individuals well, to improve our understanding of how attachment insecurity
operates in this population.
!
28!
Furthermore, much of the theorizing for this study involved a consideration of the
conceptual similarities between family systems constructs from attachment theory, such as
describing attachment anxiety and avoidance as reflective of poor differentiation of self (Bowen,
1978). Future researchers may consider exploring differentiation of self as a mediator of the
relation between interparental conflict and romantic relationship satisfaction, since a previous
study found that self-differentiation mediated the relation between experiences of family abuse
and difficulties in romantic relationships (Priest, 2015). Additionally, future research should
include constructs such as emotional cutoff, an aspect of self-differentiation in Bowen theory,
which has shown a strong association with attachment avoidance (Skowron & Dendy, 2004).
Future researchers should continue to study these constructs, replicating this study with
other, more diverse samples. In doing so, researchers could also examine the unique contribution
of each attachment dimension, since the present results and those from some previous studies
found that the dimensions operate differently in relation to couple satisfaction.
Ultimately, results of the present study suggest that for adult children of divorce,
interparental conflict is particularly harmful for adults who are high in attachment avoidance.
Future studies should also examine recall of interparental conflict from childhood in relation to
other aspects of couples’ relationships, such as level of commitment, conflict, and distress. With
an increased understanding of the experiences of adult children of high-conflict divorce,
researchers may turn their attention to testing psychotherapeutic strategies for buffering or
attenuating the long-term effects of this adverse childhood experience.
!
29!
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Amato, P. R., Loomis, L. S., & Booth, A. (1995). Parental divorce, marital conflict, and
offspring well-being during early adulthood. Social Forces, 73, 895-915.
Banse, R. (2004). Adult attachment and marital satisfaction: Evidence for dyadic configuration
effects. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 273-282.
Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a
four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244.
Bickham, N. L., & Fiese, B. H. (1997). Extension of the Children's Perceptions of Interparental
Conflict Scale for use with late adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 246-250.
Birnbaum, G. E. (2007). Attachment orientations, sexual functioning, and relationship
satisfaction in a community sample of women. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 24, 21-35.
Bowen, M. (1978) Family therapy in clinical practice. New York: Aronson.
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 99, 265-272.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavistock
Publications Limited.
!
30!
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York, NY, US:
Guilford Press.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment
relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In J. Cassidy & P. R.
Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp.
102-127). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source
of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data?. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3-5.
Butzer, B., & Campbell, L. (2008). Adult attachment, sexual satisfaction, and relationship
satisfaction: A study of married couples. Personal Relationships, 15, 141-154.
Busuito, A., Huth-Bocks, A., & Puro, E. (2014). Romantic attachment as a moderator of the
association between childhood abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder
symptoms. Journal of Family Violence, 29, 567-577.
Camisasca, E., Miragoli, S., Di Blasio, P., & Grych, J. (2017). Children’s coping strategies to
inter-parental conflict: The moderating role of attachment. Journal of Child and Family
Studies, 26, 1099-1111.
Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2005). Perceptions of conflict and
support in romantic relationships: The role of attachment anxiety. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 88, 510-531.
Cherlin, A. J. (2010). The marriage-go-round: The state of marriage and the family in America
today. New York, NY: Vintage.
!
31!
Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., & Brockmeyer, S. (2009). Parental divorce and adult children's
attachment representations and marital status. Attachment and Human Development, 11,
87-101.
Cui, M., & Fincham, F. D. (2010). The differential effects of parental divorce and marital
conflict on young adult romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 17, 331-343.
Cui, M., Fincham, F. D., & Durtschi, J. A. (2011). The effect of parental divorce on young
adults' romantic relationship dissolution: What makes a difference? Personal
Relationships, 18, 410-426.
Cui, M., Fincham, F. D., & Pasley, B. K. (2008). Young adult romantic relationships: The role of
parents' marital problems and relationship efficacy. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 34, 1226-1235.
Cusimano, A. M., & Riggs, S. A. (2013). Perceptions of interparental conflict, romantic
attachment, and psychological distress in college students. Couple and Family
Psychology: Research and Practice, 2, 45-59.
El-Sheikh, M., & Elmore–Staton, L. (2004). The link between marital conflict and child
adjustment: Parent–child conflict and perceived attachments as mediators, potentiators,
and mitigators of risk. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 631-648.
Fairchild, A. J., & Finney, S. J. (2006). Investigating validity evidence for the experiences in
close relationships-revised questionnaire. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 66, 116-135.
Feeney, J. A. (2006). Parental attachment and conflict behavior: Implications for offspring's
attachment, loneliness, and relationship satisfaction. Personal Relationships, 13, 19-36.
!
32!
Feeney, J. A., & Karantzas, G. C. (2017). Couple conflict: Insights from an attachment
perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 60-64.
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 281-291.
Ferreira, L. C., Narciso, I., Novo, R. F., & Pereira, C. R. (2014). Predicting couple satisfaction:
the role of differentiation of self, sexual desire and intimacy in heterosexual
individuals. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 29, 390-404.
Fowler, C., & Dillow, M. R. (2011). Attachment dimensions and the four horsemen of the
apocalypse. Communication Research Reports, 28, 16-26.
Fraley, R. C. (2012) Information on the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R)
Adult Attachment Questionnaire. Retrieved from
http://labs.psychology.illinois.edu/~rcfraley/measures/ecrr.htm
Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The experiences in
close relationships—Relationship Structures Questionnaire: A method for assessing
attachment orientations across relationships. Psychological Assessment, 23, 615-625.
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item-response theory analysis of self-
report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
350-365.
Funk, J. L., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). Testing the ruler with item response theory: Increasing
precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the Couples Satisfaction
Index. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 572-583.
!
33!
Gager, C. T., Yabiku, S. T., & Linver, M. R. (2016). Conflict or divorce? Does parental conflict
and/or divorce increase the likelihood of adult children's cohabiting and marital
dissolution?. Marriage and Family Review, 52, 243-261.
Gleeson, G., & Fitzgerald, A. (2014). Exploring the association between adult attachment styles
in romantic relationships, perceptions of parents from childhood and relationship
satisfaction. Health, 6, 1643-1661.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail. New York: Fireside.
Grych, J. H., & Kinsfogel, K. M. (2010). Exploring the role of attachment style in the relation
between family aggression and abuse in adolescent dating relationships. Journal of
Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 19, 624-640.
Grych, J. H., Seid, M., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Assessing marital conflict from the child's
perspective: The Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Child
Development, 63, 558-572.
Hare, A. L., Miga, E. M., & Allen, J. P. (2009). Intergenerational transmission of aggression in
romantic relationships: The moderating role of attachment security. Journal of Family
Psychology, 23, 808-818.
Hayashi, G. M., & Strickland, B. R. (1998). Long-term effects of parental divorce on love
relationships: Divorce as attachment disruption. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 15, 23-38.
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A
regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.
!
34!
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on
close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 1-22.
Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and
the Family, 50, 93-98.
Hendrick, S. S., Hendrick, C., & Adler, N. L. (1988). Romantic relationships: Love, satisfaction,
and staying together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 980-988.
Jacquet, S. E., & Surra, C. A. (2001). Parental divorce and premarital couples: Commitment and
other relationship characteristics. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 627-638.
Johnson, S., M. (2008). Couple and family therapy: An attachment perspective. In J. Cassidy &
P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical
applications (pp. 811-829). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.
Kerr, M. E., & Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation. W.W. Norton & Company.
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Davis, K. E. (1994). Attachment style, gender, and relationship stability: A
longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 502-512.
Lampis, J. (2016). Does partners’ differentiation of self predict dyadic adjustment? Journal of
Family Therapy, 38, 303-318.
Li, T., & Chan, D. K. S. (2012). How anxious and avoidant attachment affect romantic
relationship quality differently: A meta-analytic review. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 42, 406-419.
Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their
reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251-255.
Lopez, F. G. (2001). Adult attachment orientations, self–other boundary regulation, and splitting
tendencies in a college sample. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48, 440-446.
!
35!
McCarthy, G., & Taylor, A. (1999). Avoidant/ambivalent attachment style as a mediator between
abusive childhood experiences and adult relationship difficulties. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 465-477.
Moura, O., dos Santos, R. A., Rocha, M., & Matos, P. M. (2010). Children's Perception of
Interparental Conflict Scale (CPIC): Factor structure and invariance across adolescents
and emerging adults. International Journal of Testing, 10, 364-382.
Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (1997). Marital conflict and the development of infant–parent
attachment relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 152-264.
Peer, E., Samat, S., Brandimarte, L., & Acquisti, A. (2015). Beyond the Turk: An empirical
comparison of alternative platforms for crowdsourcing online behavioral research, SSRN,
Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract=2594183
Peleg, O. (2008). The relation between differentiation of self and marital satisfaction: What can
be learned from married people over the course of life? American Journal of Family
Therapy, 36, 388-401.
Platt, R. A., Nalbone, D. P., Casanova, G. M., & Wetchler, J. L. (2008). Parental conflict and
infidelity as predictors of adult children's attachment style and infidelity. American
Journal of Family Therapy, 36, 149-161.
Porter, B., & O'Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord and childhood behavior problems. Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 287-295.
Priest, J. B. (2015). A Bowen family systems model of generalized anxiety disorder and romantic
relationship distress. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 41, 340-353.
!
36!
Reese-Weber, M., & Hesson-McInnis, M. (2008). The Children's Perception of Interparental
Conflict Scale: Comparing factor structures between developmental periods. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 68, 1008-1023.
Rodrigues, L. N., & Kitzmann, K. M. (2007). Coping as a mediator between interparental
conflict and adolescents' romantic attachment. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 24, 423-439.
Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Reliability and stability of adult attachment
patterns. Personal Relationships, 1, 23-43.
Sibley, C. G., Fischer, R., & Liu, J. H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the revised Experiences
in Close Relationships (ECR-R) self-report measure of adult romantic
attachment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1524-1536.
Sibley, C. G., & Liu, J. H. (2004). Short-term temporal stability and factor structure of the
revised experiences in close relationships (ECR-R) measure of adult attachment.
Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 969-975.
Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic relationships. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 971-980.
Skowron, E. A. (2000). The role of differentiation of self in marital adjustment. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 47, 229-237.
Skowron, E. A., & Dendy, A. K. (2004). Differentiation of self and attachment in adulthood:
Relational correlates of effortful control. Contemporary Family Therapy, 26, 337-357.
Skowron, E. A., & Friedlander, M. L. (1998). The Differentiation of Self Inventory:
Development and initial validation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 235-246.
!
37!
Shi, L. (2003). The association between adult attachment styles and conflict resolution in
romantic relationships. American Journal of Family Therapy, 31, 143-157.
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of
marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15-28.
Stackert, R. A., & Bursik, K. (2003). Why am I unsatisfied? Adult attachment style, gendered
irrational relationship beliefs, and young adult romantic relationship
satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1419-1429.
Steinberg, S. J., Davila, J., & Fincham, F. (2006). Adolescent marital expectations and romantic
experiences: Associations with perceptions about parental conflict and adolescent
attachment security. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 314-329.
Straus, M.A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT)
scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88.
Timm, T. M., & Keiley, M. K. (2011). The effects of differentiation of self, adult attachment,
and sexual communication on sexual and marital satisfaction: A path analysis. Journal of
Sex and Marital Therapy, 37, 206-223.
Tucker, J. S., & Anders, S. L. (1999). Attachment style, interpersonal perception accuracy, and
relationship satisfaction in dating couples. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 25, 403-412.
Vicary, A. M., & Fraley, R. C. (2007). Choose your own adventure: Attachment dynamics in a
simulated relationship. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1279-1291.
Wei, M., Vogel, D. L., Ku, T. Y., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, affect regulation,
negative mood, and interpersonal problems: The mediating roles of emotional reactivity
and emotional cutoff. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 14-24.
!
38!
Wiseman, H., & Tishby, O. (2014). Client attachment, attachment to the therapist and client-
therapist attachment match: How do they relate to change in psychodynamic
psychotherapy? Psychotherapy Research, 24, 392-406.
Woodhouse, S. S., Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2009). Perceptions of secure base provision
within the family. Attachment & Human Development, 11, 47-67.
Yeatts, S. D., & Martin, R. H. (2015). What is missing from my missing data plan?. Stroke, 46,
130-132.
!
39!
Table 1
Participant Characteristics
Variable M SD %
Age 33.6 10.5
Gender
Man 47.1
Woman 50.9
Partner gender
Man 49.4
Woman 47.5
Relationship status
In a romantic relationship, but not married 53.5
Married 41.0
Married, but separated 1.5
Remarried 1.2
Relationship length (years) 5.41 6.86
Age at parent divorce 10.93 3.42
Legal custody after divorce
Mother 58.7
Father 11.2
Joint custody (both parents) 23.7
Grandparents 0.4
Foster care 0.3
Don’t know 2.8
Living arrangements after divorce
Mother 69.9
Father 15.0
Both parents 10.8
Grandparents 1.9
Foster care 0.3
Spouse 0.1
Other family member 0.3
Number of children 1.10 1.33
Race/ethnicity
White/Caucasian, not Latino/a/x 66.4
Asian/ Asian American 10.8
Black/African American 7.5
Hispanic/Latin American 5.3
Native American/American Indian 2.8
Biracial/Multiracial 4.6
table continues
!
40!
Table 1, cont.
Variable M SD %
Sexual orientation
Heterosexual/straight 80.5
Gay/Lesbian 3.4
Bisexual 10.5
Pansexual 2.4
Demisexual 0.1
Region of U.S.
Northeast 22.0
Midwest 20.9
Southeast 30.1
Northwest 8.1
Southwest 15.0
Education level
Some high school, no diploma 1.3
High school graduate, diploma or GED 9.4
Some college credit, no degree 21.1
Trade/technical/vocational training 4.6
Associate’s degree 11.2
Bachelor’s degree 35.5
Master’s degree 12.1
Doctoral degree 2.8
Household income
Less than $25,000 16.8
$25,000 to $34,999 13.0
$35,000 to $49,999 17.8
$50,000 to $74,999 21.4
$75,000 to $99,999 14.5
$100,000 to $149,999 9.6
$150,000 to $199,999 2.7
$200,000 or more 2.2
Note. Some percentages do not total to 100 due to missing data.
!
41!
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics on the Study Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 M SD α
1. CP -- 1.27 0.44 .89
2. CSI-4 -.04 -- 17.81 4.60 .93
3. ECR-R Anxiety .03 -.34*** -- 3.74 1.39 .94
4. ECR-R Avoidance .03 -.56*** .54*** -- 3.29 1.14 .92
Note. CP = Conflict Properties from the Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict scale
(Grych et al., 1992); CSI-4 = Couples Satisfaction Index-4 (Funk & Rogge, 2007); ECR-R =
Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (Fraley et al., 2000).
***p < .001.
!
42!
Table 3
Conditional Effects of Interparental Conflict on Romantic Relationship Satisfaction at Different
Levels of Attachment Avoidance
Avoidance %ile Effect SE t p CI
3.39 50th -.33 .35 -.95 .34 [-1.01, .34]
4.28 84th -1.01 .56 -1.82 .07 [-2.11, .08]
5.06 94th -1.36 .69 -1.96 .05 [-2.72, .00]
Note. N = 678. CI = 95%.
!
43!
Figure 1. Hypothesized Mediation Model.
Attachment
Anxiety
Interparental
Conflict
Romantic
Relationship
Satisfaction
Attachment
Avoidance
!
44!
Figure 2. Hypothesized Moderation Model.
Attachment
Anxiety
Interparental
Conflict
Romantic
Relationship
Satisfaction
Attachment
Avoidance
!
45!
Appendix A
Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale
(Grych Seid & Fincham, 1992; Bickham & Fiese, 1997)
In every family there are times when the parents are in conflict. Please respond to the following
statements with respect to how you recall your parents’ relationship when you were a child.
Specifically, think about the arguments or disagreements you might have witnessed before your
parents divorced or separated.
T = TRUE
ST = SORT OF OR SOMETIMES TRUE
F = FALSE
1. T ST F I never saw my parents arguing or disagreeing*
2. T ST F When my parents had an argument they usually worked it out*
3. T ST F My parents often got into arguments about things I did at school
4. T ST F When my parents argued I ended up getting involved somehow
5. T ST F My parents got really mad when they argued*
6. T ST F When my parents argued I could do something to make myself feel better**
7. T ST F I got scared when my parents argued**
8. T ST F I felt caught in the middle when my parents argued
9. T ST F I was not to blame when my parents had arguments
10. T ST F They may not have thought I knew it, but my parents argued or disagreed a lot*
11. T ST F Even after my parents would stop arguing, they stayed mad at each other*
12. T ST F When my parents argued I tried to do something to stop them
13. T ST F When my parents had a disagreement they discussed it quietly*
14. T ST F I didn't know what to do when my parents had arguments**
15. T ST F My parents were often mean to each other even when I was around*
16. T ST F When my parents argued I worried about what would happen to me**
!
46!
17. T ST F I didn't feel like I had to take sides when my parents had a disagreement
18. T ST F It was usually my fault when my parents argued
19. T ST F I often saw or heard my parents arguing*
20. T ST F When my parents disagreed about something, they usually came up with a
solution*
21. T ST F My parents' arguments were usually about me
22. T ST F When my parents had an argument they said mean things to each other*
23. T ST F When my parents argued or disagreed I could usually help make things better**
24. T ST F When my parents argued I was afraid that something bad would happen**
25. T ST F My mom wanted me to be on her side when she and my dad argued
26. T ST F Even if they didn't say it, I knew I was to blame when my parents argued
27. T ST F My parents hardly ever argued*
28. T ST F When my parents argued they usually made up right away*
29. T ST F My parents usually argued or disagreed because of things that I did
30. T ST F I didn't get involved when my parents argued
31. T ST F When my parents had an argument they yelled at each other*
32. T ST F When my parents argued there was nothing I could do to stop them**
33. T ST F When my parents argued I worried that one of them would get hurt**
34. T ST F I felt like I had to take sides when my parents had a disagreement
35. T ST F My parents often nagged and complained about each other around the house*
36. T ST F My parents hardly ever yelled when they had a disagreement*
37. T ST F My parents often got into arguments when I did something wrong
38. T ST F My parents broke or threw things during an argument*
!
47!
39. T ST F After my parents stopped arguing, they were friendly towards each other*
40. T ST F When my parents argued I was afraid that they would yell at me too**
41. T ST F My parents blamed me when they had arguments
42. T ST F My dad wanted me to be on his side when he and my mom argued
43. T ST F My parents pushed or shoved each other during an argument*
44. T ST F When my parents argued or disagreed there was nothing I could do to make
myself feel better**
45. T ST F When my parents argued I worried that they might get divorced**
46. T ST F My parents still acted mean after they had an argument*
47. T ST F Usually it was not my fault when my parents had arguments
48. T ST F When my parents argued they didn't listen to anything I said**
49. T ST F I have a good memory of the time when my parents were still together
50. T ST F I don’t remember much about the time when my parents were still together
51. T ST F I have clear memories of my parents’ arguments
52. T ST F I have few memories of my parents’ arguments
*Items on the Conflict Properties subscale of the Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict
Scale
**Items on the Threat subscale of the Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale
!
48!
Appendix B
Experiences in Close Relationships – Revised
(Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000)
The statements below concern how you feel in emotionally intimate relationships. We are
interested in how you generally experience relationships, not just in what is happening in a
current relationship. Respond to each statement by circling a number to indicate how much you
agree or disagree with the statement
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Neither agree nor Strongly
Disagree disagree Agree
1. I'm afraid that once a romantic partner gets to know me, he or she won't like who I
really am. (ANX)
2. I prefer not to be too close to romantic partners. (AV)
3. My partner really understands me and my needs.* (AV)
4. I find that my partner(s) don't want to get as close as I would like. (ANX)
5. My partner only seems to notice me when I’m angry. (ANX)
6. It makes me mad that I don't get the affection and support I need from my partner. (ANX)
7. It's easy for me to be affectionate with my partner.* (AV)
8. When my partner is out of sight, I worry that he or she might become interested in
someone else. (ANX)
9. I feel comfortable depending on romantic partners.* (AV)
10. I prefer not to show a partner how I feel deep down. (AV)
11. I find it difficult to allow myself to depend on romantic partners. (AV)
12. I rarely worry about my partner leaving me.* (ANX)
13. I get uncomfortable when a romantic partner wants to be very close. (AV)
14. I do not often worry about being abandoned.* (ANX)
15. I'm afraid that I will lose my partner's love. (ANX)
!
49!
16. I often worry that my partner doesn't really love me. (ANX)
17. I worry that romantic partners won’t care about me as much as I care about them. (ANX)
18. I find it easy to depend on romantic partners.* (AV)
19. I am nervous when partners get too close to me.* (AV)
20. I worry that I won't measure up to other people. (ANX)
21. I often worry that my partner will not want to stay with me. (ANX)
22. I tell my partner just about everything.* (AV)
23. I usually discuss my problems and concerns with my partner.* (AV)
24. It's not difficult for me to get close to my partner.* (AV)
25. It helps to turn to my romantic partner in times of need.* (AV)
26. I talk things over with my partner.* (AV)
27. I often wish that my partner's feelings for me were as strong as my feelings for him or
her. (ANX)
28. My desire to be very close sometimes scares people away. (ANX)
29. Sometimes romantic partners change their feelings about me for no apparent reason. (ANX)
30. I am very comfortable being close to romantic partners.* (AV)
31. I find it relatively easy to get close to my partner.* (AV)
32. I worry a lot about my relationships. (ANX)
33. I don't feel comfortable opening up to romantic partners. (AV)
34. My romantic partner makes me doubt myself. (ANX)
35. I feel comfortable sharing my private thoughts and feelings with my partner.* (AV)
36. When I show my feelings for romantic partners, I'm afraid they will not feel the same
about me. (ANX)
*Reverse scored items
!
50!
Appendix C
Couples Satisfaction Index-4 (CSI-4; Funk & Rogge, 2007)
Directions: Please select the responses that best describe your current romantic relationship
1. Please indicate the degree of happiness, all things considered, of your relationship.
0 = Extremely unhappy
1 = Fairly unhappy
2 = A little unhappy
3 = Happy
4 = Very happy
5 = Extremely happy
6 = Perfect
2. I have a warm and comfortable relationship with my partner
0 = Not at all true
1 = A little true
2 = Somewhat true
3 = Mostly true
4 = Almost completely true
5 = Completely true
3. How rewarding is your relationship with your partner?
0 = Not at all
1 = A little
2 = Somewhat
3 = Mostly
4 = Almost completely
5 = Completely
4. In general, how satisfied are you with your relationship?
0 = Not at all
1 = A little
2 = Somewhat
3 = Mostly
4 = Almost completely
5 = Completely
!
51!
Appendix D
Screening Questions
1. Are you a resident of the United States?
Yes
No
2. Are you 18 years of age or older?
Yes
No
3. Did your parents divorce when you were between the ages of 6 to 17?
Yes
No
4. Are you currently in a romantic relationship that has lasted at least 3 months?
Yes
No
!
52!
Appendix E
Demographic Questionnaire
Please respond to the following demographic questions.
1. Age:_____
2. Your gender:
Man
Woman
Transgender Man
Transgender Woman
Nonbinary/Gender nonconforming
Other (please specify)________________
Choose not to answer
3. The gender of your current romantic partner:
Man
Woman
Transgender Man
Transgender Woman
Nonbinary/Gender nonconforming
Other (please specify)________________
Choose not to answer
4. What is your current relationship status?
In a romantic relationship, but not married
Married
Married, but separated
Remarried
Other (please specify)____________________
Choose not to answer
5. How long have you been in your current romantic relationship?
_____years ______months
6. Race: Choose all that apply
Asian/Asian American
Black/African American
Hispanic/Latin American
Native American/American Indian/First Nation
Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander
White/Caucasian, not Latino/a/x
Other (please specify)______________________
Choose not to answer
!
53!
7. What is your country of origin (where you were born)_____________
7a. How long did you live there?________
8. Which region of the U.S. do you currently live in?
Northeast
Midwest
Southeast
Northwest
Southwest
9. Sexual orientation:
Heterosexual/straight
Gay/Lesbian
Bisexual
Pansexual
Other (please specify)______________________
Choose not to answer
10. How old were you when your parents divorced?:
________years old
11. Who had legal custody of you following your parents’ divorce?
Mother
Father
Joint custody (both parents)
Don’t know
Other (please specify)_______________________
12. Who did you live with most often after your parents’ divorce?
Mother
Father
Both parents
Other (please specify)____________________
13. How many children do you have?_________
14. Your highest education level:
Some high school, no diploma
High school graduate, diploma or GED
Some college credit, no degree
Trade/technical/vocational training
Associate’s degree
Bachelor’s degree
Master’s degree
Doctoral degree
!
54!
15. Your approximate household income
Less than $25,000
$25,000 to $34,999
$35,000 to $49,999
$50,000 to $74,999
$75,000 to $99,999
$100,000 to $149,999
$150,000 to $199,999
$200,000 or more
Choose not to answer
!
55!
Appendix F
Informed Consent Document
Informed Consent: Experiences of Adults Whose Parents Divorced During Childhood
This is a dissertation research study conducted by Hannah Muetzelfeld, M.Ed. (Counseling
Psychology PhD student at the University at Albany/SUNY), conducted under the supervision of
Myrna L. Friedlander, PhD (Professor of Counseling Psychology at the University at
Albany/State University of New York). This project has been approved by the University at
Albany’s Institutional Review Board. Approval of this project only signifies that the procedures
adequately protect the rights and welfare of the participants. Please note that absolute
confidentiality and anonymity cannot be guaranteed due to the limited protections of Internet
access.
Thank you for considering participating in this study. If you choose to take part in this study, you
will be asked to answer questions about your current romantic relationship, your parents’ levels
of conflict when you were a child, and some aspects of your personality. I hope to use the
knowledge gained from a large sample of adults who experienced parental divorce in childhood.
Your participation is completely voluntary and confidential. It involves completing an online
survey that should take you 25-30 minutes to complete. To thank you for taking the time to fill
out the survey, you will receive $0.25 via Mechanical Turk. Here is an overview of the study to
help you decide if you would like to continue!
To participate in this study, you must be
- 18 years or older,
- have parents who divorced when you were between the ages 6-17,
- be in a romantic relationship that has lasted 3 months or longer, and
- a resident of the United States
Even after you provide your informed consent to participate in this research, you may leave the
study at any time; however, only participants who complete the entire questionnaire will be
eligible to receive payment through MTurk. I will retain and analyze the information you have
provided up until the point you have left the study. Please note that I will be collecting IP
addresses to ensure that only participants from the U.S. are completing the questionnaires.
I anticipate that you will experience minimal risk by participating in the study. The main risk of
the research is the potential for inadvertent disclosure of personal information. Although you will
not receive any personal benefit from filling out this survey, I hope that others may ultimately
benefit from the knowledge obtained from this research.
!
56!
Due to the online nature of this study, the University at Albany’s Institutional Review Board has
waived the requirement to sign a consent form. Rather, by clicking on "Continue" below and
then proceeding to the survey on the next page, you are giving your consent to participate in this
research. You may print this page for your own records. If you do not wish to participate, simply
close out of the web browser at this time.
Please contact the principal investigator Hannah Muetzelfeld ([email protected]) or her
faculty advisor, Myrna Friedlander ([email protected]), if you have any questions
regarding the study or consent.
Your Rights as a Research Participant: Research at the University Albany involving human
participants is carried out under the oversight of the Institutional Review Board (IRB). This
research has been reviewed and approved by the IRB. If you have any questions concerning your
rights as a research subject or if you wish to report any concerns about the study, you may
contact University at Albany Office of Regulatory & Research Compliance at 1-866-857-5459
Please click “continue” to consent to participate in the study.!