JOURNAL OF OCCUPATIONAL HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY AT 20
Researching Rudeness: The Past, Present, and Future of
the Science of Incivility
Lilia M. Cortina
University of Michigan
Dana Kabat-Farr
Dalhousie University
Vicki J. Magley and Kerri Nelson
University of Connecticut
Incivility refers to rude, condescending, and ostracizing acts that violate workplace norms of respect, but
otherwise appear mundane. Organizations sometimes dismiss these routine slights and indignities—
which lack overt malice—as inconsequential. However, science has shown that incivility is a real stressor
with real consequences: though the conduct is subtle, the consequences are not. We now know a great
deal about how common incivility is, who gets targeted with it, under what conditions, and with what
effects. The first half of this article reviews and synthesizes the last 15 years of workplace incivility
research. In the second half, we look beyond that body of scholarship to pose novel questions and nudge
the field in novel directions. We also point to thorny topics that call for caution, even course correction.
Incivility in organizations is as important now as ever. Our goal is to motivate new science on incivility,
new ways to think about it and, ultimately, new solutions.
Keywords: incivility, respect, organizations, organizational behavior, occupational stress
Averaging almost one article per day, research into workplace
incivility has exploded in recent years. Incivility refers to rude,
condescending, and ostracizing acts that violate workplace norms
of respect, but otherwise appear mundane. Organizations some-
times dismiss these routine slights and indignities—which lack
overt malice—as inconsequential. When we began studying inci-
vility 20 years ago, our goal was simply to put it “on the map” as
a construct worthy of organizational inquiry. Our efforts have met
with great success, as the science of incivility has since taken off.
A Google Scholar search of the term workplace incivility returned
23 works published from the years 1996 through 2000. Contrast
that with the last half decade (2011 through 2015), which saw
1,700 articles published on this topic. How did that journey begin
and where has it taken us? In what directions should the field head
next with this topic? Likewise, where should we tread with cau-
tion? These are the questions that motivate this paper.
The impetus for our article was a piece that two of us published
in the Journal of Occupational Health Psychology (JOHP) more
than 15 years ago: “Incivility in the Workplace: Incidence and
Impact” (Cortina, Magley, Williams, & Langhout, 2001). Much to
our delight, that has proven to be our most highly cited work to
date. The present article begins by recounting the events that led up
to that publication, putting it in historical context. Next, we syn-
thesize (in broad strokes) the scholarly record that followed. That
is, we look back and chronicle the course taken by incivility
research since our “incidence and impact” article. We then look
forward to the future and chart promising new paths—and poten-
tial pitfalls—for this area of inquiry. Our article concludes with
recommendations, considerations, and cautions for the science of
incivility moving forward.
Before delving into content, we briefly note the scholarly
backgrounds of each author. Two of us (Lilia Cortina and Vicki
Magley) are what you might call “first-generation incivility
researchers,” helping establish on-the-job incivility as a topic
meriting scientific attention in North America. We began col-
laborating in the 1990s as doctoral students at the University of
Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, one in Clinical-Community Psy-
chology (Cortina) and the other in Social, Organizational, and
Individual Differences Psychology (Magley). We shared the
same advisors—Louise Fitzgerald and Fritz Drasgow—and the
same subspecializations in women’s studies and quantitative
psychology. Until then, our primary research focus had been
sexual harassment in organizations.
The other two authors (Dana Kabat-Farr and Kerri Nelson) are
“second-generation incivility researchers,” training under Cortina
and Magley. We both began studying incivility in graduate school,
one (Kabat-Farr) in Personality and Social Contexts Psychology at
the University of Michigan and the other (Nelson) in Industrial/
Lilia M. Cortina, Departments of Psychology, Women’s Studies, and
Management and Organizations, University of Michigan; Dana Kabat-Farr,
Rowe School of Business, Dalhousie University; Vicki J. Magley and Kerri
Nelson, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lilia M.
Cortina, Departments of Psychology, Women’s Studies, and Management
and Organizations, University of Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor,
MI 48109. E-mail: [email protected]
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 22, No. 3, 299 –313 1076-8998/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000089
299
Organizational Psychology at the University of Connecticut. By
the time we entered the field, more than a decade had passed since
the publication of our advisors’ 2001 JOHP article, and workplace
incivility had flourished into a vibrant line of inquiry. We use an
interdisciplinary lens to study incivility, incorporating perspectives
from occupational health psychology, social/personality psychol-
ogy, and management science (one of us, Kabat-Farr, is now
Assistant Professor of Management at Dalhousie University in
Nova Scotia, Canada).
All authors on this article have found incivility to be an exciting
area of study, and we are delighted to tell you more. Let us begin
at the beginning.
What’s Gender Got to Do With It? Remembering
Years Past
The 2001 JOHP article began as a gender story in the mid-
1990s. It emerged from a unique partnership of psychologists and
legal professionals, who joined forces to conduct research com-
missioned by the Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task Force. This
was a 30-member task force, consisting of federal judges, court
employees, practicing attorneys, professors of law, and represen-
tatives from the offices of the United States Attorneys and Federal
Public Defenders. The Task Force defined its ultimate goals as
“understanding, identifying, isolating, and eliminating gender
bias” in the federal courts (Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task
Force, 1997, p. 11). To execute the necessary research, they
engaged a social science team: psychologist Louise F. Fitzgerald
and her lab, which included Cortina and Magley (then doctoral
students).
The main focus of the Fitzgerald Lab was sexual harassment in
organizations, so we suggested to the Task Force that this be their
main focus as well. The task force agreed that sexual harassment
was a problem in their work environment, but added that incivility
was just as problematic (and perhaps more prevalent). Though
incivility in the law had never received scientific scrutiny, it was
then a focus of many commentators in legal trade publications. For
instance, attorneys were described as “modern day gladiators”
(Corr & Madden, 1995, p. 9) and “barbarians of the bar” (Pierce,
1995, p. 60). Judges were perceived as too arrogant, impatient,
impolite, and preoccupied with case management to be civil (Han-
sen, 1991; Ring, 1992). Some critics argued that the notion of
“civil litigation” had become an oxymoron (e.g., Honeywell, 1994;
Wallis-Honchar, 1997).
Reports of incivility in the legal profession, however, were
entirely anecdotal. Was the rudeness really so rampant? To address
this question, we needed an instrument to measure the incidence of
incivility. No such instrument existed, so we created one. Informed
by focus groups and conversations with attorneys, judges, and
court employees, the social science team generated items that later
became the Workplace Incivility Scale (WIS; Cortina et al., 2001).
At that point, the team still conceptualized the project as one
primarily about sexual harassment, so we thought that if nothing
else, this new instrument could function as “filler items” in our
sexual harassment survey. The new items assessed experiences of
specific, uncivil acts from superiors and coworkers over the prior
5 years. (Side note: We fought for a 1- or 2-year time window, but
task force members insisted on 5 years. Some worried that one or
two years would not be sufficient to detect incivility, assuming it
to be rare in the venerable federal court context. We have received
countless questions over the years about why we chose such a long
time-frame for the original WIS. Our answer is simple: This choice
was not ours, and if anything, we resisted it.)
Armed with our new incivility (filler) items, we sent surveys to
all judges (N 149) and all employees (N 1,167) in the Eighth
Circuit court system, as well as 4,605 randomly sampled attorneys.
The court employee data became the basis of several JOHP
articles on workplace incivility (Cortina & Magley, 2003, 2009;
Cortina et al., 2001).
1
One might wonder whether the federal court
workplace is unique—perhaps a breeding ground for incivility—
given the American adversarial model of justice. That model,
however, applies to the trying of fact, not the typical organization
and functions of court personnel (e.g., managing court documents,
supporting IT systems, accounting). Work in this context is not as
unusual as it might seem at first glance.
Following the completion of the three surveys, we rolled up our
sleeves and dove into the data. Through a stroke of serendipity, we
discovered that our “filler items” were fascinating. First, they hung
together beautifully as a reliable and cohesive scale in all three
data sets. In the employee data, we also found evidence of validity
via convergence with a climate measure of fairness (Donovan,
Drasgow, and Munson’s, 1998, Perceptions of Fair Interpersonal
Treatment Scale). Second, we found that incivility was alarmingly
common in the federal courts: More than 60% of attorneys, 70% of
employees, and 74% of judges had encountered some kind of
uncivil conduct at work during the prior 5 years (Cortina et al.,
2001, 2002). One of the district court judges on the task force
(Honorable Carol E. Jackson) captured the findings eloquently:
When we looked at interactions among the lawyers and judges, we
discovered that both groups could benefit greatly from a Miss Man-
ners course. [Incivility] was experienced across the board as disturb-
ingly high...it’s hard to say how often this kind of behavior is
intentional, or, how often it results from what my grandmother used to
call “no home training.” (Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task Force,
1997,p.8)
Third, we learned that these everyday slights and indignities
mattered in individual work lives (Cortina et al., 2001, 2002). The
more frequently people encountered incivility on the job, the less
they liked that job (i.e., lower satisfaction with work, coworkers,
supervisors, promotion opportunities, and pay and benefits). Their
symptoms of psychological distress increased, and they described
more disengagement from work as well as thoughts and intentions
of quitting. Moreover, these findings were not an artifact of a
hectic work environment; even when controlling for general job
stress, the same relationships emerged. Seemingly small acts of
incivility, we learned, were just as corrosive to work and wellbeing
as overt harassment.
After finding that this conduct was a real stressor with real
consequences, we wondered what to call it. Legal professionals
termed it incivility in their trade literature, but we discovered that
European researchers had been investigating similar phenomena
for years at that point, under different names: aggression (a term
1
For results based on the attorney and judge surveys, see Cortina et al.
(2002) and Lonsway, Freeman, Cortina, Magley, and Fitzgerald (2002),
respectively.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
300
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
used in the work of Kaj Björkqvist and colleagues), harassment
(Ståle Einarsen), bullying (Heinz Leymann), and sometimes mob-
bing (Dieter Zapf). In the first draft of the manuscript that ulti-
mately became the 2001 JOHP article, we dubbed this construct
psychological aggression, but reviewers took issue with this term.
Then, to our delight, Lynne Andersson and Chris Pearson pub-
lished their 1999 article in Academy of Management Review,
defining and theorizing this phenomenon in detail. Like our lawyer
friends, they called the conduct “incivility,” and ever since then so
have we.
The Work That Followed: 2001 Through 2016
The scientific study of workplace incivility is relatively
“young,” but it has come a long way in its short history. What has
this journey taught us? The review that follows is by no means
exhaustive; rather, we focus on major themes that emerged over
the past 15 years, starting with our 2001 JOHP article. Many
scholars have built on that work by delving further into the
incidence and impact of incivility, as well as factors that mediate
and moderate that impact. Although the literature has now grown
to encompass many related constructs, we confine our review to
articles specifically addressing “workplace incivility.” Notably, a
large majority of these studies utilized the Workplace Incivility
Scale (WIS; Cortina et al., 2001) or some adaptation of it (e.g.,
Blau & Andersson, 2005; Caza & Cortina, 2007; Cortina, Kabat-
Farr, Leskinen, Huerta, & Magley, 2013; Matthews & Ritter,
2016).
Incidence of Workplace Incivility: New Insights Into
Targets and Instigators
Targets. In our 2001 study, the great majority (70%) of
employees reported having been subjected to incivility in the prior
5 years. Among those targets, nearly one third noted that they
encountered this rudeness anywhere from “sometimes” to “many
times” (Cortina et al., 2001). Since then, research has continued to
show that incivility is common, even ubiquitous in the contempo-
rary workforce. Over time it became clear that few workers escape
incivility, regardless of function, firm, or industry. Today this
might seem obvious, but 20 years ago it was not (recall the Eighth
Circuit Task Force members who insisted on a 5-year time window
for assessing uncivil experiences, believing that one or two years
could easily elapse with no incivility at all). Even after incidence
rates were well established, questions remained: Who tends to be
most targeted with incivility, under what conditions, and why?
Also in our 2001 article, we proposed that power (e.g., based on
gender or position) would play a role in experiences of incivility,
such that those with less social power are at higher risk for being
mistreated. Indeed, we found more women than men reporting
uncivil experiences, especially in male-dominated professions
such as the law (Cortina et al., 2002). Cortina (2008) continued this
focus on power when introducing the theory of selective incivility,
which posits that incivility can act as a covert, modern manifes-
tation of gender and racial discrimination (Cortina, 2008). Testing
this theory of “selective incivility,” Cortina and colleagues (2013)
found in multiple organizations that both gender and race (and
their interaction) do relate to risk for uncivil treatment. We elab-
orate on this work in the stigmatized identities subsection below.
Notably, research has also revealed that employees need not be
directly targeted with incivility to endure its effects: witnesses are
harmed as well. For example, people are less helpful, suffer
reduced task and creative performance, and display more dysfunc-
tional ideation when witnessing acts of incivility, though these
decrements are less pronounced when the witness is in competition
with the target (Porath & Erez, 2009). Further, witnesses are more
likely to experience negative emotions when the targeted coworker
is of the same gender (Miner & Eischeid, 2012), and they perceive
more harm when the target reacts in a negative way (Chui & Dietz,
2014). Beyond personal harm, observers seek justice for such
witnessed offenses; one experimental study found that witnesses
retaliated against incivility instigators in work-related ways by
giving them undesirable work tasks and providing negative work
evaluations (Reich & Hershcovis, 2015). It is important to note
that negative affect/emotionality functioned as a mediator in many
of these studies, helping explain the negative consequences of
these observational experiences.
Instigators. Since 2001, the literature has expanded to encom-
pass the perpetration of incivility. For example, research has found
that employees who are dissatisfied or exhausted with their jobs,
have faced distributive injustice (Blau & Andersson, 2005), or
have a dominant conflict management style (Trudel & Reio, 2011)
are more likely to act uncivilly toward colleagues. Others have
examined how individual differences and job-related factors inter-
act to predict incivility instigation (e.g., Taylor & Kluemper,
2012). For example, those with an obsessive passion for their work
appear more likely to perpetrate incivility, especially in organiza-
tional climates that emphasize mastery and learning (Birkeland &
Nerstad, 2016).
A recent body of research has examined incivility instigated by
not only coworkers and supervisors but also organizational “out-
siders,” answering a call of our original 2001 JOHP article to
expand beyond “insider” instigators. Customer incivility has re-
ceived the most attention in this respect (e.g., Arnold & Walsh,
2015; Kern & Grandey, 2009; Marchiondo, Cortina, Shannon,
Haines, Geldart, & Griffith, 2015; Sliter, Jex, Wolford, & McIn-
nerney, 2010; Sliter & Jones, 2016; van Jaarsveld, Walker, &
Skarlicki, 2010; Walker, van Jaarsveld, & Skarlicki, 2014; Wilson
& Holmvall, 2013). In fact, customer and coworker incivility can
interact to influence employees’ outcomes, suggesting that those
facing incivility on multiple fronts have fewer resources at their
disposal (Sliter, Sliter, & Jex, 2012). Additionally, family mem-
bers have been examined as external instigators of incivility,
whereby incivility experienced at home has negative consequences
in the workplace (e.g., Bai, Lin, & Wang, 2016; Lim & Tai, 2014).
Researchers have developed and validated scales to measure in-
stigated (Gray, Carter, & Sears, 2017) and experienced incivility
from customers (Wilson & Holmvall, 2013), paving the way for
future investigation on these topics.
In an effort to add clarity to the term perpetrator (and victim),
Hershcovis and Reich (2013) noted that workplace aggression can
be reciprocal. In fact, research has examined both individual and
organizational factors that turn targets into instigators, contributing
to a so-called “spiral” of incivility (Andersson & Pearson, 1999).
One study found that men were more likely to perpetrate incivility
when they worked in a context that tolerated rude, disrespectful
behavior (Gallus, Bunk, Matthews, Barnes-Farrell, & Magley,
2014). Employees are also more likely to enact incivility in re-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
301
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY
sponse to experiencing incivility when working under passive
managers (Harold & Holtz, 2015). That said, Schilpzand, De Pater,
and Erez (2016) rightly noted that many of the theoretical tenets of
Andersson and Pearson’s “incivility spiral” have received little
empirical examination. Importantly, researchers have shown that
incivility instigation is not the only way in which employees
respond to incivility in the workplace, as discussed below.
Impact of Workplace Incivility: New Insights Into
When and Why
Over the past 15 years, research has consistently shown that
workplace incivility is related to negative outcomes for its targets.
The individual and organizational costs of this low-intensity be-
havior include work withdrawal (e.g., Pearson, Andersson, &
Wegner, 2001), job stress and psychological distress (e.g., Cortina
et al., 2001; Lim & Cortina, 2005), counterproductive work be-
haviors (CWBs) and lower job satisfaction (e.g., Penney & Spec-
tor, 2005), lower coworker and supervisor satisfaction (e.g., Martin
& Hine, 2005), incivility perpetration (e.g., Gallus et al., 2014; van
Jaarsveld et al., 2010), lower afterwork psychological detachment
and next-morning recovery (Nicholson & Griffin, 2015), reduced
creativity, task performance, and helpfulness (Porath & Erez,
2007), higher turnover intentions (e.g., Wilson & Holmvall, 2013),
lower marital satisfaction (Ferguson, 2012), depression, and higher
work-to-family conflict (Lim & Lee, 2011). In addition, studies
have shed light on factors that intervene in the impact of incivil-
ity—that is, mediators and moderators of the incivility-outcome
relationship. We organize this work into four categories: individual
differences, stigmatized identities, cognitive and emotional pro-
cesses, and job-related/situational factors.
Individual differences. The role of individual differences in
how employees perceive and cope with incivility has garnered
considerable attention. Beginning with perceptions, research has
shown that some people are more prone to perceiving and expe-
riencing incivility than others. For example, Bunk and Magley
(2011) suggested that certain individuals have a higher sensitivity
to interpersonal treatment, which translates into stronger reactions.
They developed and validated a scale measuring this individual
difference, opening the door for future research on mistreatment
perceptions. In a vignette study, Sliter, Withrow, and Jex (2015)
found that individuals who are higher in trait anger, conscientious-
ness, and surprisingly, positive affect are more likely to perceive
ambiguous behaviors as uncivil, whereas those higher in openness
are less likely to do so. Employees who are rated by their cowork-
ers as high in neuroticism and low in agreeableness also report
experiencing more incivility than others (Milam, Spitzmueller, &
Penney, 2009). Further, employees’ conflict management styles
may influence experiences, such that those with more dominating
styles are more likely to both encounter and engage in incivility
(Trudel & Reio, 2011).
Individual differences may also play a role in how employees
respond to uncivil behavior. For example, an external locus of
control, low emotional stability, or perceptions that others have
hostile intentions may make employees targeted with incivility
more likely to endure end-of-day negative affect (Zhou, Yan, Che,
& Meier, 2015). Employees high in neuroticism also appear more
likely to respond to a severe incivility incident by ignoring and/or
avoiding the perpetrator, whereas the opposite is true for those low
in neuroticism (Beattie & Griffin, 2014a). Further, Lim and Tai
(2014) found that employees experiencing family incivility were
more likely to suffer psychological distress when they had low
core self-evaluations; this distress, in turn, negatively affected job
performance. Ali, Ryan, Lyons, Ehrhart, and Wessel (2016) ex-
amined the moderating role of job seekers’ goal orientation on
their responses to incivility on the job market; they found greater
negative effects on job search self-efficacy and behavior when job
seekers had low “avoid-performance orientation” (that is, a low
tendency to avoid situations in which failure is a possibility). To
explain this pattern, they posited that individuals who are low in
performance-avoidant goal orientations may lack strategies to buf-
fer the effects of rejection.
Individual differences may also influence response strategies
that directly affect the organization. Using weekly experience
sampling, Taylor, Bedeian, Cole, and Zhang (2014) found that
changes in incivility frequency linked with changes in burnout and
turnover intentions, tentatively showing that these effects may be
stronger for those higher in conscientiousness. Narcissism may
also play a role, such that narcissistic individuals are more likely
to disengage and perform poorly when experiencing uncivil treat-
ment at work; a possible reason is that incivility thwarts opportu-
nities for narcissists to self-enhance (Chen et al., 2013). Using an
event-level perspective, Walker et al. (2014) found that employees
were more likely to respond to customer incivility by being uncivil
themselves when they perceived most of their interactions to be
civil. Employees may react to incivility with interpersonal devi-
ance when they believe that mistreatment should be reciprocated
and when they attribute hostile intentions to others (Wu, Zhang,
Chiu, Kwan, & He, 2014).
Stigmatized identities. Following the introduction of the the-
ory of selective incivility (Cortina, 2008), research has considered
how stigmatized identities affect workplace incivility experiences
and outcomes. In a test of the theory, Cortina et al. (2013) found
that women, people of color, and particularly African American
women reported more uncivil treatment than other groups, and
these experiences were associated with higher turnover intentions.
In a sample of conference attendees, Settles and O’Connor (2014)
also found that women (compared with men) described more
incivility at conferences. Further, although Kern and Grandey
(2009)
reported no differences in uncivil customer conduct toward
racial minority (primarily African American) versus White retail
employees, they did find that minority employees suffered more
stress from those experiences when they strongly identified with
their racial group. In contrast to these studies, Welbourne, Gan-
gadharan, and Sariol (2015) found Hispanic women describing less
incivility than either Non-Hispanic White women or Hispanic
men. Moreover, Hispanic employees demonstrated more resilience
toward negative outcomes.
Other researchers have considered the roles of factors such as
weight and motherhood status in incivility experiences. Sliter, Sliter,
Withrow, and Jex (2012) found that individuals meeting criteria for
being overweight or obese reported higher levels of incivility, and this
effect was moderated by race and gender. Additionally, in a study
conducted by Miner, Pesonen, Smittick, Seigel, and Clark (2014),
incivility experiences were more frequent for mothers of three chil-
dren compared with those with fewer children or none at all. Moth-
erhood, however, also served a buffering or protective role against
negative outcomes. Further, Porath, Overbeck, and Pearson (2008)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
302
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
reported that employees’ coping strategies differed according to both
gender and status. Specifically, male targets, and those of higher
status, were more likely to respond aggressively to incivility, espe-
cially when the instigator was of equal status. In contrast, low-status
and female targets were more likely to distance themselves or avoid
the instigator.
Cognitive and emotional mechanisms. Other studies have
explored how incivility takes a toll by way of cognitive and emotional
processes. For example, research has concluded that incivility impairs
task performance and engagement by disrupting cognitive processes
(e.g., memory; Porath & Erez, 2007) or depleting mental, emotional,
and social energy (Giumetti, Hatfield, Scisco, Schroeder, Muth, &
Kowalski, 2013). Kabat-Farr, Cortina, and Marchiondo (2016) found
negative affect and guilt in response to incivility linking to decreased
empowerment and self-esteem as well as increased withdrawal; emo-
tional reactions were especially pronounced for the most committed
employees. Emotional processes may also help connect workplace
incivility to nonwork outcomes. One study found that workplace
incivility fueled hostile emotions, especially for those higher in trait
hostility; these emotions, in turn, predicted angry and withdrawn
behavior at home (Lim, Ilies, Koopman, Christoforou, & Arvey,
2016).
Further, these processes may work together to predict employees’
coping strategies, like support seeking, conflict avoidance, aggression,
assertion, and absenteeism (Cortina & Magley, 2009; Porath & Pear-
son, 2012). In a sample of university students, Caza and Cortina
(2007) found that incivility experiences related positively to percep-
tions of injustice and social ostracism, which in turn predicted lower
institutional satisfaction and higher psychological distress. Research
has also examined emotional labor, emotional exhaustion, and job
demands as mechanisms influencing relationships among customer
incivility, employee-perpetrated incivility, and customer service qual-
ity (Sliter et al., 2010; van Jaarsveld et al., 2010). Negative emotions
and optimism may also mediate links between incivility and such job
outcomes as job satisfaction, work effort, and CWBs (Bunk &
Magley, 2013; Sakurai & Jex, 2012).
Third parties can also influence—and be influenced by— cognitive
and emotional responses to incivility. Schilpzand, Leavitt, and Lim
(2016) found a buffering effect, whereby targets who perceived they
were not alone in their experiences (i.e., witnessed a team member
being treated uncivilly) had lower appraisals of self-blame and, in
turn, less stress, rumination, and withdrawal. Foulk, Woolum, and
Erez (2016) suggest that incivility may be “contagious” by activating
concepts related to rudeness in the target’s mind and carrying over
into subsequent encounters with others.
Job-related and situational factors. Moving beyond the in-
dividual, research has demonstrated that both job-related and sit-
uational factors may exacerbate the negative effects of incivility.
For example, Welbourne and Sariol (2017) found that employees
with high job involvement were more likely to respond to incivility
by engaging in CWBs. They also reported that female targets with
jobs requiring higher task interdependence were more likely to
engage in CWBs, but the opposite was true for male targets.
Further, Sliter and Jones (2016) found that both physical aspects of
the work environment (e.g., disorganization, cleanliness) and qual-
ities of the employees themselves (e.g., unprofessional interaction,
neuroticism) play roles in incivility instigated by customers. Cam-
eron and Webster (2011) also examined incivility as a potential
consequence of “multicommunicating” (e.g., juggling more than
one conversation simultaneously) at work.
In contrast, both professional and personal resources can buffer
the negative effects of incivility. For example, Walsh and col-
leagues (2012) found that employees who perceive their work-
group as having a civil climate report fewer incivility experiences.
Further, transformational leadership (Arnold & Walsh, 2015)as
well as social/emotional support at work (Beattie & Griffin, 2014b;
Miner, Settles, Pratt-Hyatt, & Brady, 2012) and home (Lim & Lee,
2011) can attenuate negative outcomes of incivility. Job control
and psychological detachment can also reduce the impact of daily
e-mail incivility on end-of-workday distress and (subsequently)
next-morning distress (Park, Fritz, & Jex, 2015). However, inci-
vility can also undermine professional resources; Miner-Rubino
and Reed (2010) found that workgroup incivility predicted turn-
over intentions, burnout, and lower job satisfaction indirectly, via
decreases in organizational trust.
Notably, incivility can have effects at both the workgroup and
organizational levels. Lim, Cortina, and Magley (2008) found that
workgroup-level incivility predicted turnover intentions and health
impairments beyond individual experiences of incivility, and this
was mediated by (declines in) mental health and job satisfaction.
Further, Griffin (2010) found that organization-level incivility
explained additional variance in intentions to stay after accounting
for individual experiences.
In summary, the last 15 years have witnessed great strides in our
understanding of the incidence and impact of workplace incivility.
However, it is important to acknowledge that the majority of this
research on incivility’s impact has been based on cross-sectional,
correlational designs. Consistent with theoretical assumptions un-
dergirding this literature (e.g., stressors trigger strains, incivility
saps resources), we have used implicitly causal and temporal
language throughout this section (e.g., “impact”) with the caveat
that causal and temporal conclusions must remain tentative until
confirmed using other research designs. Recent work has started
filling these gaps, including workplace incivility experiments (e.g.,
Foulk et al., 2016; Giumetti et al., 2013; Hershcovis & Reich,
2013; Porath & Erez, 2007, 2009; Reich & Hershcovis, 2015;
Schilpzand et al., 2016) and daily diary survey designs (e.g.,
Beattie & Griffin, 2014a, 2014b; Nicholson & Griffin, 2015; Park
et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2015). For example, studies utilizing diary
designs have provided tentative support for the conclusion that a
small number of incidents, even over a small timeframe, have
measurable consequences for employees such as reduced next-day
recovery and well-being. Further, Park et al. (2015) speculate that
findings from daily diary studies may be an “early warning sign”
of more serious outcomes to come. We eagerly anticipate further
advances in the literature, using these designs and others, to
continue deepening our understanding of the incidence and impact
of workplace incivility.
The preceding sections look back and synthesize the scholarly
record to date. We now shift to looking forward to the future of
incivility science. Many questions remain unanswered and many
methods unexplored; the next sections highlight topics that seem
especially ripe for further inquiry. We also note potential areas of
concern that call for caution (and, in some cases, course correc-
tion).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
303
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY
Uncharted Waters: Promising Pathways for
Incivility Science
Beyond the dyad. Incivility research to date has largely taken
an individual target/instigator approach, conceptualizing one indi-
vidual for each role. Problematizing this perspective (Fox & Spec-
tor, 2005; Hershcovis & Reich, 2013), scholars have argued that
roles should be viewed as fluid, wherein an individual might be
targeted with incivility at one point, instigate incivility at another,
and later become a target once more. Recent work is starting to
push the discussion past the dyad, referring to the process of
incivility spreading like a contagious disease (Foulk et al., 2016).
The dyadic approach also limits our ability to understand how
incivility functions amid a network of actors. A network perspec-
tive could open up new and important empirical questions. In
particular, it may help us track which individuals within a network
are “senders” of rude behavior (behaving badly toward others),
“receivers” (on the receiving end), or reciprocators (exchanging
similar acts of insult; Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006). Social
network analysis would also expand the ways in which we con-
ceptualize and operationalize some familiar concepts, such as
power. We could study the role of formal organizational power,
centrality (the degree of importance of an individual in a network),
and social power (see Raven, Schwarzwald, & Koslowsky, 1998),
as well as their intersections with various identities (e.g., race,
gender, and class). For example, we could consider the intersec-
tions of experience as determined by multiple positions of power
(or lack thereof): a female manager’s encounter with incivility
when she has a low degree of centrality in her network versus a
male’s experience in a similar organizational position with high
centrality.
Network analysis also enables group-level examination, allow-
ing us to consider the role of cliques and subgroups in facilitating
or inhibiting incivility. Cohesive subgroups of individuals may be
more likely to view others in the network as members of an
outgroup and use incivility to demarcate group boundaries. Groups
may be formed on the basis of shared social identity, similar
organizational status, or common views on organizational politics.
These groups may or may not be readily apparent to organizational
members, much less researchers, but might come to light through
social network analysis. It may be, for example, that rampant
incivility among restaurant employees is best understood through
a lens of group dynamics between front-house and back-of-house
employees.
Further, network analyses could enable studies of incivility at
the organization level, in order to detect how organizational con-
text affects incivility. The literature to date draws on samples from
a variety of industries, but does little to reconcile the various
organizational norms and structures in which participants are em-
bedded. A meso-level approach is necessary to compare and con-
trast the incivility phenomenon in relation to these norms and
cultures.
Network analysis has the potential to both answer new questions
as well as more thoroughly test existing ideas in the literature. For
example, network analysis can help tease apart issues of incidence
(at both the individual and group level) and impact. A method-
ological limitation of behavioral scales such as the WIS (Cortina et
al., 2001) is that they do not distinguish between reports of a few
behaviors many times versus many behaviors a few times. Social
network analysis allows measurement of number and frequency of
incivility experiences, enabling a finer-grained calculation of in-
civility and its effects over time for a particular target, including
comparisons of incidence-impact relationships for different indi-
viduals within the network. With this method, we may uncover
information as to when infrequent exposure to incivility matters
and whether there exists a threshold effect: an amount of incivility
after which deleterious outcomes emerge for the target or others in
the network. Further, a longitudinal network analysis would more
fully test the idea of incivility functioning as a contagion, increas-
ing exposure of incivility across people and time. Finally, social
network analysis could also enable a test of the tit-for-tat theory of
incivility (Andersson & Pearson, 1999), examining the process
through which targets may or may not reciprocate uncivil actions.
Incivility researchers interested in conducting social network stud-
ies should look to related mistreatment literatures for guidance,
including school bullying (e.g., Huitsing, Snijders, Van Duijn, &
Veenstra, 2014), ostracism (e.g., Yang & Treadway, 2016), and
cyberaggression (Wegge, Vandebosch, Eggermont, & Walrave,
2015).
Beyond the spiral. Andersson and Pearson (1999) offered the
metaphor of a “spiral” to conceptualize a tit-for-tat progression of
uncivil events on the job. As we summarized in our 2001 JOHP
article, their theory proposes that
incivility can represent the beginning of an upward spiral of negative
organizational events, eventually escalating to coercive and violent
employee behavior. [Andersson and Pearson] suggest that the accu-
mulation of a series of low-level, aggravating encounters leads to a
“tipping point,” when the last minor injustice triggers intense, retal-
iatory aggression....Thus, relatively minor forms of interpersonal
mistreatment can, over time, precipitate major organizational conflict.
(Cortina et al., 2001,p.65)
According to this reasoning, incivility begets incivility, aggres-
sion, and violence. Pointing to research in criminology, Andersson
and Pearson (1999, p. 458) give the example that, “one person
mocks another; the second responds with an obscene insult. The
first shoves; the second hits. And the conflict escalates until one
person is seriously wounded.”
Andersson and Pearson (1999) proposed the incivility spiral as
a possible chain of events, but we know from empirical research
that it is not a common one. Physical violence, after all, is rare in
organizations (Schat, Frone, & Kelloway, 2006). To their credit,
Andersson and Pearson (1999) acknowledged that the spiraling of
incivility into an “exchange of coercive actions” is “relatively
infrequent” (p. 462), observing that there are points when any of
the parties involved can depart from the uncivil exchange. For
example, targets can ignore the behavior, reinterpret it as benign or
unintentional, or walk away. They can also recognize the rudeness
for what it is, but take no action in response. Or instigators can
apologize, prompting forgiveness from the target. In these scenar-
ios, the disrespect never “spirals” into aggression, violence, or
anything of the sort. Andersson and Pearson (1999) wrote explic-
itly about these points of departure from the incivility spiral, and
this was clear in their graphical depiction of a sample spiral
(Figure 3 in Andersson & Pearson, 1999, p. 460). However,
subsequent research has all but ignored the notion of departure
points. As of this writing, a Google Scholar search of the terms
workplace incivility and departure points returns all of three pub-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
304
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
lications (one of which we authored: Kabat-Farr et al., 2016). In
contrast, when we replace departure points with spiral, we find
more than 500 scholarly works.
We recommend that researchers take seriously the notion of
departure points and forge new insights into the incivility-response
cycle. If spirals into coercive aggression and violence are rare,
what are some more typical behavioral reactions to incivility on
the job? How do people commonly respond to rudeness within
their work relationships, and how can we make sense of those
reactions? These questions require new theory and new interdis-
ciplinary thinking. In short, we suggest that the metaphor of the
incivility spiral may have run its course. It has been useful in
capturing the rare incident of incivility that escalates into violence,
drawing theoretical and practical attention to this topic. Let us now
build on that foundational work with new propositions, new con-
cepts, and new results pertaining to incivility in the workplace.
Beyond the critical incident. Like other areas of organiza-
tional science, workplace incivility research is grappling with the
question, “What is the appropriate temporal lens for this topic?” As
noted earlier, the WIS asks respondents to provide retrospective
accounts of the frequency of their experiences, looking back over
the past 5 years (original 7-item WIS; Cortina et al., 2001)orone
year (newer 12-item version; Cortina et al., 2013). Many studies
follow these general accounts with a series of detailed questions
about a critical incident (e.g., asking about its duration, perpetra-
tors, appraisal, coping; Bunk & Magley, 2013; Cortina & Magley,
2009). More recently, some researchers have moved beyond this
critical incident approach and collected true longitudinal incivility
data—via either event-contingent sampling or interval-contingent
sampling (cf., Meier & Gross, 2015; Rosen, Koopman, Gabriel, &
Johnson, 2016; see Cole, Shipp, & Taylor, 2016 for a summary).
We applaud these efforts, as conducting such research, quite
simply, is difficult. However, it is time (so to speak) to contem-
plate the value of different temporal approaches to the science of
incivility.
To begin, the typical retrospective recall approach to assessing
incivility does allow researchers to capture broad-stroke connec-
tions between experiences and outcomes. In other words, such
studies are the basis for understanding that experiences of incivil-
ity carry both professional and personal costs, as already reviewed.
The importance of this research should not be ignored: before its
accumulation, the idea that such seemingly small, low-level, am-
biguous behaviors could really affect workers was dismissed as
absurd. Also, retrospective accounts allow for incidence and prev-
alence estimates. Although varying temporal “lookback” periods
make it difficult to compare such estimates directly, it would be
even more difficult to generalize incidence estimates from event-
based or daily diary accounts of such experiences.
Longitudinal methods are important in isolating causal and
dynamic relationships. As Cole and colleagues conclude, “the
adoption of a purely static or ‘snapshot’ approach toward inter-
personal mistreatment masks considerable and meaningful fluctu-
ations in the experience of, responses to, and consequences of such
behavior” (2016). Further, they offer insightful guidance—not
repeated here for lack of space—in the construction of such
longitudinal studies.
Although we look forward to continued longitudinal contribu-
tions to this literature, we also want to balance our enthusiasm with
three considerations. First, virtually all of the recent prospective
incivility research involves interval-contingent sampling (Beal &
Weiss, 2003). That is, it is still retrospective; the retrospective time
period is simply shortened from (usually) one year to one day/
week/month with repeated measurements the next day/week/
month. How frequently and over what duration this assessment
occurs strike us as blind endeavors, likely as driven by construct
demands as they are by financial limitations and/or student-
progress realities. One challenge to the literature, given these
varied temporal frames, is that it is quite difficult to make direct
comparisons across studies. As Ford and colleagues (2014) dem-
onstrate, time lags do affect the strength of associations between
stressors and strains. We suggest that article abstracts include all
temporal details of study designs, not only to be very explicit
up-front, but also to facilitate comparisons as studies accumulate.
Additionally, we recommend that researchers take care to provide
ample descriptive information about the frequency of incivility
detected in such longitudinal studies, again to facilitate compari-
sons.
Second, we submit that the sheer nature of incivility—with its
ambiguity and low-level nature—may well make longer time
frames more appropriate than shorter ones. Interpersonal ex-
changes are not always, immediately— or within even a day or
so—recognizable as involving a behavior problematic enough to
note on a survey. Considerations of the context, the people in-
volved and how they typically behave, and the behavior in ques-
tion may require time to process; likewise, understanding of the
situation as rude or inappropriate could also take time. Of course,
some experiences are obvious, but many are not (by definition,
incivility is ambiguous). It remains an empirical question what
factors influence rapid identification.
Finally, we must always keep in mind that longitudinal studies
often necessitate truncated measurement. Rather than respond to a
comprehensive list of behaviors in the repeated surveys, partici-
pants may be asked to respond to an abbreviated scale or single
item, again requiring rapid interpretation. As behavioral detail gets
lost, so does precision. It is important that we weigh the benefits of
repeated measurement against the costs of truncated measurement.
Clearly, more research is needed to understand incivility through a
temporal lens. Until then, we caution against the assumption that
longitudinal methods are always best for answering the increas-
ingly complex questions arising in this area.
Beyond the negative. Research has concluded that incivility
is both common and costly to organizations, with costs including
lost revenue (Porath & Pearson, 2013). The vast number of schol-
arly articles on the topic have included an equally vast number of
“practical implications” sections. However, few ideas about prac-
tical interventions have been put to an empirical test. What should
organizations do to stem the tide of incivility or curb its insidious
spread?
To combat incivility, scientists and practitioners have developed
civility interventions, many with great success. Cisco Systems,
Inc., was one of the first corporations to introduce a civility
promotion training program (Pearson & Porath, 2009). Through
workshops, case studies, coaching, and video presentations, em-
ployees and managers learn to both recognize and address incivil-
ity. Cisco’s culture of mutual respect complements these formal
training activities. In the public sphere, the National Institute for
Civil Discourse promotes healthy, civil political debate in the
United States, linking incivility research to public practice. Addi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
305
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY
tionally, organizational scholars (Spence Laschinger, Leiter, Day,
Gilin-Oore, & Laschinger, 2012) have documented the success of
the Civility, Respect, and Engagement in the Workplace (CREW)
intervention, finding workgroups that implement the 6-month pro-
gram to have lower supervisor incivility, higher civility percep-
tions, and more positive job attitudes (e.g., commitment, self-
efficacy) one year later.
Examining these successful civility interventions may help us
understand the mechanisms of harm in an uncivil workplace. For
example, CREW, which aims to both reduce incivility and pro-
mote civility, includes weekly or biweekly meetings with a trained
facilitator to set goals and promote positive teamwork (Spence
Laschinger et al., 2012). At the center of these meetings is the
formation of respectful and trusting relationships among members
(Spence Laschinger et al., 2012), pointing to the importance of
shared positive workgroup norms. The success of this relational
approach to fostering civility also suggests that notions of reci-
procity and positive emotional contagion may be at play (Leiter,
Day, Oore, & Spence Laschinger, 2012). Another civility inter-
vention, the Civility Among Healthcare Professionals project, fo-
cuses on themes of community, engagement, and empowerment in
efforts to enhance the quality of the social environment at work
(Graham, Zweber, & Magley, 2013; Walsh & Magley, 2013). A
much shorter term intervention than CREW, these train-the-trainer
2-hr sessions are conducted in small groups and can foster imme-
diate changes in knowledge and attitudes surrounding workplace
civility (Graham et al., 2013; Walsh & Magley, 2013). These
intervention examples focus on civil behaviors and discourse, but
what is civility?
The science of workplace civility is just beginning to take off. In
their 1999 article, Andersson and Pearson conceptualized civility
as a manner of conveying respect, cooperation, and moral stan-
dard; they noted that business has been often a place where civility
is manifested as “formality yet friendliness, distance yet polite-
ness” (p. 453). Other scholars define civil behavior as treating
others with dignity, respecting others’ feelings, and upholding
social norms for mutual respect (Carter, 1998; Johnson, 1988).
More recently, Porath, Gerbasi, and Schorch (2015) conceptual-
ized civility as being treated with respect, dignity, politeness, and
pleasantness. Workplace civility includes behaviors that do not
necessarily warrant public documentation or notice (Van Dyne,
Cummings, & McLean Parks, 1995).
We contend that civility lies on a 2-dimensional spectrum of
interpersonal organizational behavior, as illustrated in Figure 1.
One dimension captures impact on performance, from enhancing
to degrading. The second dimension entails a range from low
intensity/high ambiguity to high intensity/low ambiguity. Crossing
these two dimensions, we find four quadrants of interpersonal
organizational behavior. Civil conduct falls into the performance-
enhancing, low-intensity, high-ambiguity quadrant.
Civil behaviors, similar to incivility, are low-level and ambig-
uous in their intent (see Figure 1). Individuals who engage in
civility do so because it is “the right thing to do” (Andersson &
Pearson, 1999), with no intention to benefit the target or organi-
zation (Pearson et al., 2001). From the target or observer’s per-
spective, civil behaviors may not be overt in their positive intent.
They may not even be particularly memorable. Someone held the
elevator for you, listened to your contributions during a meeting,
said “thank you” at the end of their email: none of these actions are
extraordinary. Civility involves respectful, polite behaviors that
most of us would desire in our working environment.
Burgeoning research on civility in organizations shows promise:
civil individuals benefit from their positive actions. Porath et al.
(2015) found that individuals who perceive a colleague to be civil
are more likely to ask him/her for work advice and to perceive
her/him as a leader. Greater perceptions of civility by one’s net-
work are associated with improved performance on account of
perceptions of warmth and competence. In other words civility,
though subtle, can be performance-enhancing. Its impact on per-
formance may be smaller than more extraordinary acts of inter-
Performance
ce
-
e
-
enhancing Performance
ce
-
e
-
degrading
Civility
i
vi
l
it
y
Courtesy
Incivility
I
nciv
i
li
t
y
Microaggressions
Low intensity/High ambiguity High intensity/Low ambiguity
Interpersonal
al
cizenship behavior
son
a
al
i
c
z
enship
b
Social support
ocia
l
sup
p
o
Helping
Hel
Posive
ng
el
pi
e
e
deviance
Aggression
A
g
g
r
e
s
sion
Bullying
Bul
l
ying
Harassment
H
a
r
assme
n
t
Ostracism
Figure 1. Two-dimensional spectrum of interpersonal organizational behavior.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
306
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
personal citizenship (Kabat-Farr & Cortina, 2017); we suggest this
possibility in Figure 1—with civility positioned closer to neutral
on the performance dimension, compared with more extraordinary
positive behaviors— but this proposition requires empirical test-
ing.
Civility has also been conceptualized at the organizational and
team levels as “workgroup norms for civility” (Walsh, Magley,
Reeves, Davies-Schrils, Marmet, & Gallus, 2012), and early re-
search indicates that it can improve safety climate, reduce work-
place injuries (McGonagle, Walsh, Kath, & Morrow, 2014), and
reduce interpersonal deviance in the face of organizational con-
straints (Clark & Walsh, 2016). This emerging research, it is
interesting to note, focuses on norms of interpersonal treatment,
which is one area that the incivility field is relatively silent on
(despite norm violation being a key distinguishing factor of inci-
vility as defined by Andersson & Pearson, 1999). As this schol-
arship moves forward, it will be important to test the “antidote”
ability of a civility climate, especially in organizations that have
established and pungent incivility norms.
Civility is associated with certain positive outcomes, but should
it be the ultimate goal? Or should organizations strive for more? A
civil climate serves as the foundation to positive relationships and
empathy at work (Pearson, Andersson, & Porath, 2000). We en-
courage research that builds on civil foundations to branch farther
into the positive realm, fostering behaviors such as interpersonal
citizenship characterized as positive “above and beyond” helping,
cooperation, consideration (Kabat-Farr & Cortina, 2017; Settoon
& Mossholder, 2002).
The spectrum of workplace behaviors detailed in Figure 1
conceptualizes conduct at an individual level. However, (in)civil-
ity interventions generally focus on the group level, in which there
may be both good and bad actors. To combat incivility, we suggest
focusing on positive workplace actions, but we caution that it takes
many more positive behaviors to overcome the negative (Baumeis-
ter, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001). Because of this,
efforts to eliminate rudeness should be in place as well as efforts
to encourage the positive. At the same time, we also should be
cognizant of the implications and complexities of encouraging
positive behavior. Research by Eagly (2009) details gender differ-
ences in pro-social behavior, both in the performance of and
stereotypes about such actions. Women tend to be more relational
(establishing close personal, dyadic relationships), whereas men
tend to enact prosocial behaviors that aid the organization or group
(Eagly, 2009). Both forms of pro-social behavior should be en-
couraged and rewarded, keeping in mind what we know about
gender and helping behavior: helping behavior may be seen as
“in-role” for women (consistent with gender stereotypes of women
and communal) and “extra-role” for men, resulting in these be-
haviors being less noticed and less rewarded for women (Allen &
Rush, 2001; Caleo, 2016; Lovell et al., 1999). At the same time,
when men do help, their behaviors may be attributed to self-
serving motivations, resulting in devaluation (Farrell & Finkel-
stein, 2007). However, there is also research to suggest that men’s
altruistic helping behaviors on work tasks are rewarded through
positive evaluations and recommendations, whereas women’s are
not (Heilman & Chen, 2005). Although we want to spur helping
behaviors, we must be mindful of how these behaviors intersect
with social norms and identities.
In sum, civility may be a springboard for more intense positive
actions. Research has demonstrated the benefits of such acts for
those who engage in them (i.e., focusing on the actor). We en-
courage researchers to delve also into the receiving end of inter-
personal citizenship behaviors; such an approach could enable us
to document how positive behaviors function as social resources
for employees. Early research in this area suggests promising
effects of being targeted with interpersonal citizenship behavior,
including increased job satisfaction (Regts & Molleman, 2013),
thriving, empowerment, and performance of organizational citi-
zenship (Kabat-Farr & Cortina, 2017). This evidence indicates that
organizations may be able to foster gains through the building of
social resources via positive interactions (e.g., Hakanen, Perhoni-
emi, & Toppinen-Tanner, 2008).
Troubled Waters: Proceed With Caution
The dark side of civility. The preceding section frames civil-
ity in a positive light, but not all scholars share this sunny view of
civility in organizations. If we look beyond the confines of our
discipline (psychology), we find thought leaders in other fields
expressing alarm about civility interventions. Intriguing questions
emerge: does civility have a dark side? Where might we find
problems in this work and how can we correct them? Is it possible
to reconcile the pro- and anti-civility perspectives?
These questions came to a head in August 2014, when the firing
(or “un-hiring”) of Steven Salaita at the University of Illinois at
Urbana–Champaign ignited heated debate nationwide about inci-
vility in (academic) employment. Top university executives issued
calls for civility and respect. For example, in his September 2014
inaugural address, University of Michigan President Mark Schlis-
sel expressed aspirations for “Michigan to be known as a place
where mutual respect does not require agreement, where differ-
ences of perspective are treated with sensitivity, and where we all
become advocates for, and experts in, civil discourse.” That same
month, UC Berkeley Chancellor Nicholas Dirks messaged his
campus that: “We can only exercise our right to free speech insofar
as we feel safe and respected in doing so, and this in turn requires
that people treat each other with civility” (see Flaherty, 2014).
According to these and other leaders, civility is a prerequisite for
the free and open exchange of ideas—it is vital for a strong
academy. However, other scholars are condemning such calls for
civility as regressive tools of censorship.
According to some academics—often in the humanities—civil-
ity has a dark underbelly that works against free and critical speech
(e.g., Flaherty, 2014). For instance, UCLA history professor Mi-
chael Meranze (2014) maintains that “the repetitive invocation of
‘civil’ and ‘civility’ to set limits to acceptable speech bespeaks a
broader and deeper challenge to intellectual freedom.” These cri-
tiques are often referring to attempts by those at the top to silence
dissident voices below them, in the name of civility. In this sense,
the discourse of “civility” structures and is structured by power.
According to historian Joan Scott (2015), “the notion of civility
consistently establishes relations of power whenever it is invoked.
Moreover, it is always the powerful who determine its meaning—
one that, whatever its specific content, demeans and delegitimizes
those who do not meet its test.” In other words, people in power
can define what constitutes civility and incivility, and then relegate
their critics to the dust-heap of the latter. Echoing similar senti-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
307
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY
ments, media studies scholar Andrew Calabrese (2015, p. 540)
shows how civility can be “wielded as a weapon to limit, silence
or otherwise control the free expression of the weak.” Once
branded as uncivil, one loses credibility and rationality in the eyes
of others, and one’s critical ideas lose purchase (Scott, 2015).
These scholars caution that dangerous agendas hide behind appeals
to civility, especially when those appeals come from above.
We should add that these problems are not confined to aca-
demia: across a variety of industries, calls for civility can have the
effect of suppressing unpopular speech or minority opinions. One
can imagine a corporate board meeting in which members with
different ideas about strategic planning acquiesce to the views of
those in power, scared of being accused of rudeness should they
oppose the dominant majority or challenge the status quo. In this
way, civility mandates can make employees feel unsafe to speak
up and express divergent perspectives. This self-silencing can be
bad for people and bad for organizations, stifling innovation and
diversity of thought.
The “perils of civility” argument stands in contrast to the theory
of selective incivility (Cortina, 2008; see also Cortina et al., 2013;
Kabat-Farr & Cortina, 2012). This perspective views incivility as
an instrument of oppression, used to ostracize women, people of
color, and other undervalued minorities from organizational life
(Cortina, 2008). Note that an analysis of power also figures prom-
inently in this work: unequal distributions of power pervade our
society, and “asymmetrical power combined with prejudice sets
the stage” for selective incivility on the job (Cortina, 2008, p. 62).
Moreover, experts in workplace mistreatment (not only incivility
but also harassment, bullying, abusive supervision, and the like)
often advocate civility and respect as progressive goals to strive
for— essential components of a healthy, hostility-free work envi-
ronment (e.g., Feldblum & Lipnic, 2016; Leiter, Laschinger, Day,
& Oore, 2011; Yamada, 2010). Civil and respectful conduct can-
not, by definition, be hostile conduct.
In our view, there is merit in both the anti- and pro-civility
standpoints. Rather than being incompatible, perhaps they are
confronting different realities. The humanists stress that their aims
are not to “claim that all appeals to civility are inherently fraud-
ulent and valueless” (Calabrese, 2015, p. 550) or encourage “the
warfare of the uncivil” (Scott, 2015). Instead they urge critical
analysis of (in)civility, especially through a lens of power; we
could not agree more. Two important questions emerge from this
work: (1) who is invoking civility, and (2) why?
Scott, Calabrese, Meranze, and others protest calls for civility
issued by the powerful (answering the “who” question) to control,
discipline, and silence dissident minorities (the “why” question).
In contrast, occupational health psychologists promote calls for
civility issued by stakeholders at all levels (including but not
limited to leadership) for the purpose of protecting workforce
health and wellbeing; the objective is to create dignified working
conditions for all persons, especially those in the minority (Cor-
tina, 2008; Cortina et al., 2013; Kabat-Farr & Cortina, 2012).
These are different sets of actors, pursuing different goals. One
could easily agree with both viewpoints, so perhaps they are not so
irreconcilable after all.
The diverging perspectives on civility point to interesting but
thorny questions: Is one employee’s right to free (but potentially
harassing or hateful) speech in conflict with another’s right to
respect at work? In the specific context of higher education, should
we defend faculty sexism, racism, or homophobia in the name of
academic freedom? Should we use “civility” as a criterion in
selection and promotion decisions; if so, how should we go about
doing that, and how can we make certain that civility goals do not
act as cover for prejudice? In short, this controversy reveals a
potential dark side to civility: when used to suppress voice, criti-
cism, or anger over social ills. It also exposes a bright side to some
incivility: when used to protest social injustice, sometimes loudly
and angrily. Both possibilities, we suggest, deserve attention. The
challenge here is that “once we destabilize the binary of good–
civility versus bad–incivility, we must contend with a much more
elusive set of demons” (Calabrese, 2015, p. 541). This takes us into
new territory, marked with both promise and peril. Proceed with
caution.
To help incivility researchers make sense of this difficult terrain,
we urge scholars to bridge their disciplinary divides. Many hu-
manists, alarmed about the dark side of civility, seem unaware of
the volumes of organizational research on this topic. Likewise,
many organizational scientists know nothing about the critical
discussions of civility impinging on academic freedom that are
cropping up in the humanities. We recommend more interdisci-
plinary conversations and collaborations in this domain. Partner-
ships across social science disciplines, and between social scien-
tists and humanists, could yield innovative insights into problems
of (in)civility in organizations. Additionally, we advocate that such
discussions not exclude another oftentimes ignored stakeholder in
academia— university staff. Akin to the court employees in our
2001 JOHP article, their work environment is also entrenched in
the cultural norms of the academy.
The trouble with siloes. The workplace mistreatment litera-
ture is now bursting with an array of different constructs. At first
glance, this may seem a positive development, signaling a surge of
interest in these topics. The research is becoming segmented into
siloes, however, as each scholar works narrowly on her/his favorite
construct. We urge researchers to resist this trend. Instead, it is
important that we read widely across diverse literatures, diverse
disciplines, and diverse forms of interpersonal mistreatment. This
can help open up new avenues of inquiry and avoid reinventing old
ones. To illustrate, we offer examples of three related literatures
(on cyberbullying, ostracism, and sexual harassment) that could
inform the science of incivility moving forward.
Bullying, and in particular cyberbullying (bullying carried out
using mobile phones or the Internet; e.g.,
Smith et al., 2008), lives
in a distinct literature from incivility; however, we see great
potential to inform the science of incivility. For example, incivility
research to date has been centered on face-to-face interactions (for
important exceptions see Giumetti et al., 2013; Lim & Teo, 2009;
Park, Fritz, & Jex, 2015), but given the increased use of telecom-
muting and the integration of technology, it may be more likely to
occur virtually. The cyberbullying literature has pointed to unique
features of cyber mistreatment that might also be pertinent for
incivility researchers to consider. First, digital mistreatment takes
place where there is a lack of supervision (Patchin & Hinduja,
2006), making it more difficult to detect. Second, instigators are
sometimes protected by the anonymity of cyberspace (Kowalski &
Limber, 2007), making it difficult to identify the perpetrator.
Finally, cyber mistreatment may thrive on account of the accessi-
bility of targets (Slonje, Smith, & Frisén, 2013). Workplace mis-
treatment is no longer limited to a 9-to-5 office, but continues long
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
308
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
after the workday, making it difficult to escape. As the cyberbul-
lying literature grows, incivility scholars may find it helpful to
look there for guidance.
Social ostracism is another related literature that incivility re-
searchers may find useful, in particular the work that examines
physiological consequences. For instance, social ostracism can
trigger engagement in unethical behavior, and physiological
arousal (measured using galvanic skin response) acts as a key
mechanism in this relationship (Kouchaki & Wareham, 2015).
Additionally, McQuaid and colleagues used a psychobiological
approach to uncover a genetic basis for responses to interpersonal
environments, and found that differences in the oxytocin receptor
gene may make some people more sensitive to negative social
stressors than others (McQuaid, McInnis, Matheson, & Anisman,
2015). The incivility literature has so far been relatively silent on
the biological front (for an exception see Giumetti et al., 2013, who
found no significant relationship between simulated supervisor
cyber incivility and heart rate), leaving many interesting questions
open for future study.
Incivility scientists should seek to bridge research on identity-
ambiguous (e.g., incivility, ostracism) and identity-salient mis-
treatment (sexual harassment, racial/ethnic harassment, heterosex-
ist harassment, etc.). Housed in separate literatures, these varieties
of misbehavior are conceptualized as separate and distinct, when in
reality they are highly correlated (e.g., Lim & Cortina, 2005) and
sometimes one and the same (see Cortina, 2008). Moreover, un-
derstandings of identity-salient mistreatment could advance our
understandings of incivility. The sexual harassment literature, for
instance, is much older than incivility science. Harassment spe-
cialists have dealt extensively with organizational prevention and
remediation strategies, which could inform interventions designed
around workplace incivility. Sexual harassment scientists have
also been in conversation with experts in employment law; this
could provide insights into how to think about legal implications of
workplace incivility. Studies of sexual harassment outside of North
America have started to yield a cross-cultural understanding of
gender and aggression in organizations; this work can help us think
about how cultural values and norms influence incivility on the
job. These are just a few examples. Our point is that we must resist
the temptation of insularity, and instead realize the benefits of
cross-pollination of ideas from disparate literatures, disciplines,
and mistreatment domains.
Steering clear of myths. Looking back over the past 15 years,
we raise a point that returns us back to our 2001 JOHP article,
which focused on the incidence and impact of workplace incivility.
As is clear from the present review, that paper laid the foundation
for considerable—and consistent—research highlighting that
workplace incivility is abundant and of consequence. Most empir-
ical articles, in fact, begin with a brief overview of this set of facts
prior to introducing the unique contribution of that article’s re-
search. Such introductions are certainly understandable, but, given
the considerable and consistent evidence, they beg the question of
their continued purpose. In other words, why is it that we, re-
searchers, feel the need to continue articulating these relation-
ships?
We believe that the answer to this question lies in an anticipated
disbelief from the audience about the pervasiveness and serious-
ness of incivility. This anticipated disbelief has, at least for us,
been very real and only marginally lessened over the years, despite
the accumulated evidence to the contrary. We routinely hear com-
ments like the following: Aren’t people making too much out of
simple slights, exaggerating what’s happened? Could people be
“claiming” mistreatment in an effort to protect themselves from
possible (deserved) organizational sanctions? Aren’t people being
a bit overly sensitive? Is it really that big of a deal?
Such comments reflect attitudes and beliefs that parallel those
expressed as core myths about sexual violence. Specifically, these
myths have been defined as “attitudes and beliefs that are generally
false but are widely and persistently held, and that serve to deny
and justify male sexual aggression” in the form of rape (Lonsway
& Fitzgerald, 1994, p. 134) and sexual harassment (Lonsway,
Cortina, & Magley, 2008, p. 600). Pervasive in our society, these
fallacies deny the reality of sexual violence and, when denial
becomes impossible, justify it. We suggest that similar mytholo-
gies may surround all forms of interpersonal mistreatment—inci-
vility included—in which power dynamics, doubt of victim harm,
and suppression of perpetrator culpability loom large.
What happens with dismissive attitudes such as these is an
empirical question. And, we argue, one that is worth asking. At an
individual level, we anticipate that such attitudes might undermine
victims’ desire to report their experiences. In other words, if
victims themselves question the nature of their experience(s), it
seems highly unlikely that they would seek assistance from others,
either organizationally or personally. At a group level, with shared
sentiment that, for example, “a bit of animosity can fuel creativ-
ity,” we anticipate that group functionality might suffer. Of course,
these ideas are speculative and require careful investigation. At
this point, we hope to encourage researchers to consider how
myths such as these might function to impede victims’ well-being.
Perhaps most important, however, we hope that such myths not
shape future research choices.
Closing Thoughts
The science of incivility has traveled far during its short life.
Since generating the (filler) items comprising the original Work-
place Incivility Scale (Cortina et al., 2001), we have learned a great
deal about incidence rates, impacts, boundary conditions, media-
tors, moderators, and more. This article builds on that work by
reviewing it, critiquing it, and looking beyond it. We pose novel
questions and nudge the field in novel directions. We also point to
thorny topics that call for caution, even course correction. Incivil-
ity (and civility) in organizations is as important now as ever. Our
goal is to motivate new science on incivility, new ways to think
about it and, ultimately, new solutions.
References
Ali, A. A., Ryan, A. M., Lyons, B. J., Ehrhart, M. G., & Wessel, J. L.
(2016). The long road to employment: Incivility experienced by job
seekers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 333–349. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/apl0000055
Allen, T. D., & Rush, M. C. (2001). The influence of ratee gender on
ratings of organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 31, 2561–2587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001
.tb00191.x
Andersson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect
of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24,
452– 471.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
309
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY
Arnold, K. A., & Walsh, M. M. (2015). Customer incivility and employee
well-being: Testing the moderating effects of meaning, perspective
taking and transformational leadership. Work and Stress, 29, 362–378.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2015.1075234
Bai, Q., Lin, W., & Wang, L. (2016). Family incivility and counterpro-
ductive work behavior: A moderated mediation model of self-esteem
and emotional regulation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 94, 11–19.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2016.02.014
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001).
Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
Beal, D. J., & Weiss, H. M. (2003). Methods of ecological momentary
assessment in organizational research. Organizational Research Meth-
ods, 6, 440 464. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428103257361
Beattie, L., & Griffin, B. (2014a). Accounting for within-person differ-
ences in how people respond to daily incivility at work. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 87, 625– 644. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/joop.12067
Beattie, L., & Griffin, B. (2014b). Day-level fluctuations in stress and
engagement in response to workplace incivility: A diary study. Work and
Stress, 28, 124 –142.
Birkeland, I. K., & Nerstad, C. (2016). Incivility is (not) the very essence
of love: Passion for work and incivility instigation. Journal of Occupa-
tional Health Psychology, 21, 77–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0039389
Blau, G., & Andersson, L. (2005). Testing a measure of instigated work-
place incivility. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
ogy, 78, 595– 614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/096317905X26822
Bunk, J. A., & Magley, V. J. (2011). Sensitivity to interpersonal treatment
in the workplace: Scale development and initial validation. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 84, 395– 402. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1348/096317910X488626
Bunk, J. A., & Magley, V. J. (2013). The role of appraisals and emotions
in understanding experiences of workplace incivility. Journal of Occu-
pational Health Psychology, 18, 87–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0030987
Calabrese, A. (2015). Liberalism’s disease: Civility above justice. Euro-
pean Journal of Communication, 30, 539 –553. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0267323115595306
Caleo, S. (2016). Are organizational justice rules gendered? Reactions to
men’s and women’s justice violations. Journal of Applied Psychology,
101, 1422–1435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000131
Cameron, A., & Webster, J. (2011). Relational outcomes of multicommu-
nicating: Integrating incivility and social exchange perspectives. Orga-
nization Science, 22, 754 –771. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1100.0540
Carter, S. L. (1998). Civility: Manners, morals, and the etiquette of
democracy. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Caza, B. B., & Cortina, L. M. (2007). From insult to injury: Explaining the
impact of incivility. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29, 335–350.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01973530701665108
Chen, Y., Ferris, D. L., Kwan, H. K., Yan, M., Zhou, M., & Hong, Y.
(2013). Self-love’s lost labor: A self-enhancement model of workplace
incivility. Academy of Management Journal, 56, 1199–1219. http://dx
.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0906
Chui, C. W. S., & Dietz, J. (2014). Observing workplace incivility towards
women: The roles of target reactions, actor motives, and actor-target
relationships. Sex Roles, 71, 95–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-
014-0367-7
Clark, O. L., & Walsh, B. M. (2016). Civility climate mitigates deviant
reactions to organizational constraints. Journal of Managerial Psychol-
ogy, 31, 186 –201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JMP-01-2014-0021
Cole, M. S., Shipp, A. J., & Taylor, S. G. (2016). Viewing the interpersonal
mistreatment literature through a temporal lens. Organizational Psychol-
ogy Review, 6, 273–302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2041386615607095
Corr, K. P., & Madden, P. M. (1995). Goodbye, Rambo—Hello, Mr.
Rogers? For the Defense, 37, 8 –14.
Cortina, L. M. (2008). Unseen injustice: Incivility as modern discrimina-
tion in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 33, 55–75.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2008.27745097
Cortina, L. M., Kabat-Farr, D., Leskinen, E. A., Huerta, M., & Magley,
V. J. (2013). Selective incivility as modern discrimination in organiza-
tions: Evidence and impact. Journal of Management, 39, 1579–1605.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206311418835
Cortina, L. M., Lonsway, K. A., Magley, V. J., Freeman, L. V., Col-
linsworth, L. L., Hunter, M., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). What’s gender
got to do with it? Incivility in the federal courts. Law & Social Inquiry,
27, 235–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-4469.2002.tb00804.x
Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2003). Raising voice, risking retaliation:
Events following interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace. Journal
of Occupational Health Psychology, 8, 247–265. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/1076-8998.8.4.247
Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2009). Patterns and profiles of response
to incivility in the workplace. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol-
ogy, 14, 272–288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014934
Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001).
Incivility in the workplace: Incidence and impact. Journal of Occupa-
tional Health Psychology, 6, 64 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-
8998.6.1.64
Donovan, M. A., Drasgow, F., & Munson, L. J. (1998). The Perceptions of
Fair Interpersonal Treatment Scale: Development and validation of a
measure of interpersonal treatment in the workplace. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 83, 683– 692. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.83.5
.683
Eagly, A. H. (2009). The his and hers of prosocial behavior: An exami-
nation of the social psychology of gender. American Psychologist, 64,
644 658. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.64.8.644
Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task Force. (1997). Final report and
recommendations of the Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task Force.
Creighton Law Review, 31, 1–181.
Farrell, S. K., & Finkelstein, L. M. (2007). Organizational citizenship
behavior and gender: Expectations and attributions for performance.
North American Journal of Psychology, 9, 81–95.
Feldblum, C. R., & Lipnic, V. A. (2016). EEOC Select Task Force on the
Study of Harassment in the Workplace: Report of the Co-Chairs. Re-
trieved from https://www.eeoc.gov
Ferguson, M. (2012). You cannot leave it at the office: Spillover and
crossover of coworker incivility. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
33, 571–588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.774
Flaherty, C. (2014, September 9). The problem with civility. Inside Higher
Ed. Retrieved from https://www.insidehighered.com/news/2014/09/09/
berkeley-chancellor-angers-faculty-members-remarks-civility-and-free-
speech
Ford, M. T., Matthews, R. A., Wooldridge, J. D., Mishrac, V., Kakard,
U. M., & Strahan, S. R. (2014). How do occupational stressor-strain
effects vary with time? A review and meta-analysis of the relevance of
time lags in longitudinal studies. Work & Stress, 28, 9 –30.
Foulk, T., Woolum, A., & Erez, A. (2016). Catching rudeness is like
catching a cold: The contagion effects of low-intensity negative behav-
iors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 50 67. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/apl0000037
Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (Eds.). (2005). Counterproductive work behavior:
Investigations of actors and targets. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10893-000
Gallus, J. A., Bunk, J. A., Matthews, R. A., Barnes-Farrell, J. L., &
Magley, V. J. (2014). An eye for an eye? Exploring the relationship
between workplace incivility experiences and perpetration. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 19, 143–154. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0035931
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
310
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
Giumetti, G. W., Hatfield, A. L., Scisco, J. L., Schroeder, A. N., Muth,
E. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (2013). What a rude e-mail! Examining the
differential effects of incivility versus support on mood, energy, engage-
ment, and performance in an online context. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 18, 297–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032851
Graham, L. A., Zweber, Z. M., & Magley, V. J. (2013, April). Proximal
and distal outcomes of a workplace civility training. Paper presented at
the annual meeting for the Society for Industrial and Organizational
Psychology, Houston, TX.
Gray, C. J., Carter, N. T., & Sears, K. L. (2017). The UWBQ-I: An
adaption and validation of a measure of instigated incivility. Journal of
Business and Psychology, 32, 21–39.
Griffin, B. (2010). Multilevel relationships between organizational-level
incivility, justice and, intention to stay. Work and Stress, 24, 309 –323.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2010.531186
Hakanen, J. J., Perhoniemi, R., & Toppinen-Tanner, S. (2008). Positive
gain spirals at work: From job resources to work engagement, personal
initiative and work-unit innovativeness. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
73, 78 –91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2008.01.003
Hansen, M. (1991). Incivility a problem, survey says. ABA Journal, 77, 22.
Harold, C. M., & Holtz, B. C. (2015). The effects of passive leadership on
workplace incivility. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36, 16 –38.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.1926
Heilman, M. E., & Chen, J. J. (2005). Same behavior, different conse-
quences: Reactions to men’s and women’s altruistic citizenship behav-
ior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 431– 441. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0021-9010.90.3.431
Hershcovis, M. S., & Reich, T. C. (2013). Integrating workplace aggres-
sion research: Relational, contextual, and method considerations. Jour-
nal of Organizational Behavior, 34, S26 –S42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
job.1886
Honeywell, M. G. (1994). How to be on the offensive without being
offensive. Trial, 30, 86.
Huitsing, G., Snijders, T. A., Van Duijn, M. A., & Veenstra, R. (2014).
Victims, bullies, and their defenders: A longitudinal study of the coevo-
lution of positive and negative networks. Development and Psychopa-
thology, 26, 645– 659. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000297
Johnson, C. (1988). Socially controlled civility: The functioning of rituals
in the divorce process. American Behavioral Scientist, 31, 685–701.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0002764288031006007
Kabat-Farr, D., & Cortina, L. M. (2012). Selective incivility: Gender, race,
and the discriminatory workplace. In S. Fox & T. Lituchy (Eds.), Gender
and the dysfunctional workplace (pp. 107–119). Northhampton, MA:
Edward Elgar. http://dx.doi.org/10.4337/9780857932600.00014
Kabat-Farr, D., & Cortina, L. M. (2017). Receipt of interpersonal citizen-
ship: Fostering agentic emotion, cognition, and action in organizations.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 47, 74 89. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/jasp.12421
Kabat-Farr, D., Cortina, L. M., & Marchiondo, L. A. (2016). The emotional
aftermath of incivility: Anger, guilt, and the role of organizational
commitment. International Journal of Stress Management. Advance
online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/str0000045
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kern, J. H., & Grandey, A. A. (2009). Customer incivility as a social
stressor: The role of race and racial identity for service employees.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 46 –57. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0012684
Kouchaki, M., & Wareham, J. (2015). Excluded and behaving unethically:
Social exclusion, physiological responses, and unethical behavior. Jour-
nal of Applied Psychology, 100, 547–556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0038034
Kowalski, R. M., & Limber, S. P. (2007). Electronic bullying among
middle school students. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 41,
22–30.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.08.017
Leiter, M. P., Day, A., Oore, D. G., & Spence Laschinger, H. K. (2012).
Getting better and staying better: Assessing civility, incivility, distress,
and job attitudes one year after a civility intervention. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 17, 425– 434. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0029540
Leiter, M. P., Laschinger, H. K. S., Day, A., & Oore, D. G. (2011). The
impact of civility interventions on employee social behavior, distress,
and attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 1258 –1274. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/a0024442
Lim, S., & Cortina, L. M. (2005). Interpersonal mistreatment in the
workplace: The interface and impact of general incivility and sexual
harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 483– 496. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.483
Lim, S., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Personal and workgroup
incivility: Impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 93, 95–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.95
Lim, S., Ilies, R., Koopman, J., Christoforou, P., & Arvey, R. D. (2016).
Emotional mechanisms linking incivility at work to aggression and
withdrawal at home: An experience-sampling study. Journal of Man-
agement. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0149206316654544
Lim, S., & Lee, A. (2011). Work and nonwork outcomes of workplace
incivility: Does family support help? Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 16, 95–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021726
Lim, S., & Tai, K. (2014). Family incivility and job performance: A
moderated mediation model of psychological distress and core self-
evaluation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 351–359. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0034486
Lim, V. K., & Teo, T. S. (2009). Mind your E-manners: Impact of cyber
incivility on employees’ work attitude and behavior. Information &
Management, 46, 419 425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2009.06.006
Lonsway, K. A., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Sexual harass-
ment mythology: Definition, conceptualization, and measurement. Sex
Roles, 58, 599 615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9367-1
Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1994). Rape myths: In review.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 133–164. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb00448.x
Lonsway, K. A., Freeman, L. V., Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., &
Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). Understanding the judicial role in addressing
gender bias: A view from the Eighth Circuit Federal Court System. Law
& Social Inquiry, 27, 205–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-4469
.2002.tb00803.x
Lovell, S. E., Kahn, A. S., Anton, J., Davidson, A., Dowling, E., Post, D.,
& Mason, C. (1999). Does gender affect the link between organizational
citizenship behavior and performance evaluation? Sex Roles, 41, 469
478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018883018719
Marchiondo, L. A., Cortina, L. M., Shannon, H. S., Haines, T., Geldart, S.,
& Griffith, L. (2015). Service with a smile meets customer with a snarl:
Links between customer incivility and worker wellbeing. In M. Paludi
(Ed.), Bullies in the workplace (pp. 150 –172). New York, NY: Praeger.
Martin, R. J., & Hine, D. W. (2005). Development and validation of the
Uncivil Workplace Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 10, 477–490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998
.10.4.477
Matthews, R. A., & Ritter, K. J. (2016). A concise, content valid, gender
invariant measure of workplace incivility. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 21, 352–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000017
McGonagle, A. K., Walsh, B. M., Kath, L. M., & Morrow, S. L. (2014).
Civility norms, safety climate, and safety outcomes: A preliminary
investigation. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 19, 437– 452.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037110
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
311
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY
McQuaid, R. J., McInnis, O. A., Matheson, K., & Anisman, H. (2015).
Distress of ostracism: Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism confers
sensitivity to social exclusion. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci-
ence, 10, 1153–1159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu166
Meier, L. L., & Gross, S. (2015). Episodes of incivility between subordi-
nates and supervisors: Examining the role of self-control and time with
an interaction-record diary study. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
36, 1096 –1113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.2013
Meranze, M. (2014). The order of civility. Retrieved from http://
utotherescue.blogspot.com/2014/09/the-order-of-civility.html
Milam, A. C., Spitzmueller, C., & Penney, L. M. (2009). Investigating
individual differences among targets of workplace incivility. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 58 69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0012683
Miner, K. N., & Eischeid, A. (2012). Observing incivility toward cowork-
ers and negative emotions: Do gender of the target and observer matter?
Sex Roles, 66, 492–505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0108-0
Miner, K. N., Pesonen, A. D., Smittick, A. L., Seigel, M. L., & Clark, E. K.
(2014). Does being a mom help or hurt? Workplace incivility as a
function of motherhood status. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol-
ogy, 19, 60 –73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034936
Miner, K. N., Settles, I. H., Pratt-Hyatt, J. S., & Brady, C. C. (2012).
Experiencing incivility in organizations: The buffering effects of emo-
tional and organizational support. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
42, 340 –372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00891.x
Miner-Rubino, K., & Reed, W. D. (2010). Testing a moderated mediational
model of workgroup incivility: The roles of organizational trust and
group regard. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 3148 –3168.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00695.x
Nicholson, T., & Griffin, B. (2015). Here today but not gone tomorrow:
Incivility affects after-work and next-day recovery. Journal of Occupa-
tional Health Psychology, 20, 218 –225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0038376
Park, Y., Fritz, C., & Jex, S. M. (2015). Daily cyber incivility and distress:
The moderating roles of resources at work and home. Journal of Man-
agement. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0149206315576796
Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2006). Bullies move beyond the schoolyard:
A preliminary look at cyberbullying. Youth Violence and Juvenile Jus-
tice, 4, 148 –169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1541204006286288
Pearson, C. M., Andersson, L. M., & Porath, C. L. (2000). Assessing and
attacking workplace incivility. Organizational Dynamics, 29, 123–137.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0090-2616(00)00019-X
Pearson, C. M., Andersson, L. M., & Wegner, J. W. (2001). When workers
flout convention: A study of workplace incivility. Human Relations, 54,
1387–1419. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00187267015411001
Pearson, C., & Porath, C. (2009). The cost of bad behavior: How incivility
is damaging your business and what to do about it. London, England:
Penguin.
Penney, L. M., & Spector, P. E. (2005). Job stress, incivility, and coun-
terproductive work behavior (CWB): The moderating role of negative
affectivity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 777–796. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1002/job.336
Pierce, J. L. (1995). Gender trials: Emotional lives in contemporary law
firms. Berkeley, Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2007). Dues rudeness really matter? The effects
of rudeness on task performance and helpfulness. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 50, 1181–1197. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/20159919
Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2009). Overlooked but not untouched: How
rudeness reduces onlookers’ performance on routine and creative tasks.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 29 44.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.01.003
Porath, C. L., Gerbasi, A., & Schorch, S. L. (2015). The effects of civility
on advice, leadership, and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,
100, 1527–1541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000016
Porath, C. L., Overbeck, J. R., & Pearson, C. M. (2008). Picking up the
gauntlet: How individuals respond to status challenges. Journal of Ap-
plied Social Psychology, 38, 1945–1980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1559-1816.2008.00375.x
Porath, C. L., & Pearson, C. M. (2012). Emotional and behavioral re-
sponses to workplace incivility and the impact of hierarchical status.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(S1), E326 –E357. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.01020.x
Porath, C., & Pearson, C. (2013). The price of incivility. Harvard Business
Review, 91, 114 –146.
Raven, B. H., Schwarzwald, J., & Koslowsky, M. (1998). Conceptualizing
and measuring a power/interaction model of interpersonal influence.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 307–332. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01708.x
Regts, G., & Molleman, E. (2013). To leave or not to leave: When
receiving interpersonal citizenship behavior influences an employee’s
turnover intention. Human Relations, 66, 193–218. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0018726712454311
Reich, T. C., & Hershcovis, M. S. (2015). Observing workplace incivility.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 203–215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0036464
Ring, L. M. (1992). The incivility crisis. Trial, 29, 78.
Rosen, C. C., Koopman, J., Gabriel, A. S., & Johnson, R. E. (2016). Who
strikes back? A daily investigation of when and why incivility begets
incivility. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1620 –1634. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/apl0000140
Sakurai, K., & Jex, S. M. (2012). Coworker incivility and incivility targets’
work effort and counterproductive work behaviors: The moderating role
of supervisor social support. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol-
ogy, 17, 150 –161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027350
Schat, A. C. H., Frone, M. R., & Kelloway, E. K. (2006). Prevalence of
workplace aggression in the U.S. workforce: Findings from a national
study. In E. K. Kelloway, J. Barling, & J. J. Hurrell (Eds.), Handbook of
workplace violence (pp. 47–89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. http://dx
.doi.org/10.4135/9781412976947.n4
Schilpzand, P., De Pater, I. E., & Erez, A. (2016). Workplace incivility: A
review of the literature and agenda for future research. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 37(S1), S57–S88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
job.1976
Schilpzand, P., Leavitt, K., & Lim, S. (2016). Incivility hates company:
Shared incivility attenuates rumination, stress, and psychological with-
drawal by reducing self-blame. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 133, 33– 44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016
.02.001
Schlissel, M. (2014). Inaugural address: The power of ideas and the value of
all voices. Retrieved http://president.umich.edu/news-communications/
speeches/inaugural-address-the-power-of-ideas-and-the-value-of-all-voices/
Scott, J. W. (2015). The new thought police: Why are campus administra-
tors invoking civility to silence critical speech? Retrieved from https://
www.thenation.com/article/new-thought-police/
Settles, I. H., & O’Connor, R. C. (2014). Incivility at academic confer-
ences: Gender differences and the mediating role of climate. Sex Roles,
71, 71– 82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-014-0355-y
Settoon, R. P., & Mossholder, K. W. (2002). Relationship quality and
relationship context as antecedents of person- and task-focused interper-
sonal citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 255–267.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.255
Sliter, K. A., Sliter, M. T., Withrow, S. A., & Jex, S. M. (2012). Employee
adiposity and incivility: Establishing a link and identifying demographic
moderators and negative consequences. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 17, 409 424.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029862
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
312
CORTINA, KABAT-FARR, MAGLEY, AND NELSON
Sliter, M., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J. (2010). How rude!
Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and employee
outcomes. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15, 468 481.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020723
Sliter, M., & Jones, M. (2016). A qualitative and quantitative examination
of the antecedents of customer incivility. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 21, 208 –219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039897
Sliter, M., Sliter, K., & Jex, S. (2012). The employee as a punching bag:
The effect of multiple sources of incivility on employee withdrawal
behavior and sales performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
33, 121–139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.767
Sliter, M., Withrow, S., & Jex, S. M. (2015). It happened, or you thought
it happened? Examining the perception of workplace incivility based on
personality characteristics. International Journal of Stress Management,
22, 24 45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038329
Slonje, R., Smith, P. K., & Frisén, A. (2013). The nature of cyberbullying,
and strategies for prevention. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 26
32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.05.024
Smith, P. K., Mahdavi, J., Carvalho, M., Fisher, S., Russell, S., & Tippett,
N. (2008). Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school
pupils. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 376 –385.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01846.x
Spence Laschinger, H. K., Leiter, M. P., Day, A., Gilin-Oore, D., &
Mackinnon, S. P. (2012). Building empowering work environments
that foster civility and organizational trust: Testing an intervention.
Nursing Research, 61, 316 –325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/NNR
.0b013e318265a58d
Taylor, S. G., Bedeian, A. G., Cole, M. S., & Zhang, Z. (2014). Developing
and testing a dynamic model of workplace incivility change. Journal of
Management, 43, 645– 670.
Taylor, S. G., & Kluemper, D. H. (2012). Linking perceptions of role stress
and incivility to workplace aggression: The moderating role of person-
ality. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 17, 316 –329. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028211
Trudel, J., & Reio, T. G., Jr. (2011). Managing workplace incivility: The
role of conflict management styles—Antecedent or antidote? Human
Resource Development Quarterly, 22, 395– 423. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1002/hrdq.20081
Van Dyne, L., Cummings, L. L., & McLean Parks, J. (1995). Extra-role
behaviors: In pursuit of construct and definitional clarity (a bridge over
muddied waters). In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in
organizational behavior (Vol. 17, pp. 215–285). Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.
van Jaarsveld, D. D., Walker, D. D., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2010). The role of
job demands and emotional exhaustion in the relationship between
customer and employee incivility. Journal of Management, 36, 1486
1504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206310368998
Walker, D. D., van Jaarsveld, D. D., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2014). Exploring
the effects of individual customer incivility encounters on employee
incivility: The moderating roles of entity (in)civility and negative affec-
tivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 151–161. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0034350
Wallis-Honchar, C. (1997). Right to remain silent can quiet incivility.
Chicago Daily Law Bulletin, 143, 5.
Walsh, B. M., & Magley, V. J. (2013, May). Testing a model of civility
training effectiveness. Paper presented at the symposium Work, Stress
and Health, Los Angeles, CA.
Walsh, B. M., Magley, V. J., Reeves, D. W., Davies-Schrils, K. A.,
Marmet, M. D., & Gallus, J. A. (2012). Assessing workgroup norms for
civility: The development of the civility norms questionnaire-brief.
Journal of Business and Psychology, 27, 407– 420. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/s10869-011-9251-4
Wegge, D., Vandebosch, H., Eggermont, S., & Walrave, M. (2015). The
strong, the weak, and the unbalanced: The link between tie strength and
cyberaggression on a social network site. Social Science Computer
Review, 33, 315–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0894439314546729
Welbourne, J. L., Gangadharan, A., & Sariol, A. M. (2015). Ethnicity and
cultural values as predictors of the occurrence and impact of experienced
workplace incivility. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 20,
205–217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038277
Welbourne, J. L., & Sariol, A. M. (2017). When does incivility lead to
counterproductive work behavior? Roles of job involvement, task inter-
dependence, and gender. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
22, 194 –206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000029
Wilson, N. L., & Holmvall, C. M. (2013). The development and validation
of the Incivility From Customers Scale. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 18, 310 –326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032753
Wu, L., Zhang, H., Chiu, R. K., Kwan, H. K., & He, X. (2014). Hostile
attribution bias and negative reciprocity beliefs exacerbate incivility’s
effects on interpersonal deviance. Journal of Business Ethics, 120,
189 –199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1658-6
Yamada, D. C. (2010). Workplace Bullying and American Employment
Law: A Ten-Year Progress Report and Assessment. Comparative Labor
Law & Policy Journal, 32, 251.
Yang, J., & Treadway, D. C. (2016). A social influence interpretation of
workplace ostracism and counterproductive work behavior. Journal of
Business Ethics. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10551-015-2912-x
Zhou, Z. E., Yan, Y., Che, X. X., & Meier, L. L. (2015). Effect of
workplace incivility on end-of-work negative affect: Examining individ-
ual and organizational moderators in a daily diary study. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 20, 117–130. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0038167
Received November 28, 2016
Revision received April 11, 2017
Accepted May 12, 2017
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
313
SCIENCE OF INCIVILITY